Data publikacji: 19 wrz 2025
DOI: https://doi.org/10.2478/fprj-2025-0005
Słowa kluczowe
© 2025 Sonya Lutter et al., published by Sciendo
This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
Psychology, the study of the mind, is increasingly shaping the practice of financial planning. Historically, financial advice focused primarily on quantitative analysis and technical strategies, often overlooking the psychological and emotional dimensions of financial decision making. However, failing to consider how individuals think and feel about financial recommendations limits the effectiveness of financial planning interventions. Recognizing this gap, the Certified Financial Planner Board of Standards, Inc. added the psychology of financial planning as a competency domain, reinforcing the need for financial professionals to understand the underlying drivers of client behaviour (CFP Board 2024). With this evolution comes a need for financial planners to integrate psychological principles—particularly those that enhance financial wellness—into their practice.
Well-being tends to encompass satisfaction and other subjective indicators of feeling content and fulfilled about areas of one’s life (Global Wellness Institute n.d.). The Consumer Finance Protection Bureau defined financial well-being as, “a condition wherein a person can fully meet current and ongoing financial obligations, can feel secure in their financial future, and is able to make choices that allow them to enjoy life” (Consumer Financial Protection Bureau 2017). Research suggests a correlation between financial well-being and broader life satisfaction (Kim & Chatterjee 2019; Korankye & Kalenkoski 2021), physical and mental health and relationship satisfaction (Brüggen
Positive psychology, a field pioneered by Seligman (2011), emphasizes wellness or flourishing as a key outcome in a fulfilling and meaningful life. Positive psychology interventions delivered via individual, group or self-help/educational methods seem to be helpful in increasing wellness and reducing stress, anxiety and depression (Carr
Positive framing is a technique often applied within positive psychology. Evidence from Fagley
Application of Positive Psychology
Exercise optimism and hope | Acknowledge progress with encouraging statements: “The progress you have made is impressive and really speaks to your commitment to your values and goals”. |
Savour/appreciate the present moment | Set electronics aside and engage in focused conversation with clients. |
Identifying and using strengths | Reinforce strengths with specific feedback: ‘Your consistency demonstrates your focus and strong discipline, which will continue to support your success’. |
Practice kindness | Suggest ways clients can integrate giving into their financial plans, for example, allocating a portion for charitable donations or supporting others, to reinforce a sense of purpose. |
Note meaningful experiences | Encourage clients to document their meaningful financial achievements or experiences that brought them joy or fulfilment, like successfully paying off debt or achieving a savings milestone. |
Practice meaning making | Highlight the purpose behind financial actions: ‘Increasing your retirement contributions not only strengthens your financial future but also aligns with your long-term goal of ensuring financial security, allowing you to travel and spend time with your grandchildren’. |
While there is general agreement that individuals’ mindset about money influences their behaviour, there is less understanding of
Research by Medvec
This social comparison is particularly prevalent in the digital age. Vogel
While financial professionals traditionally focus on finding objective indicators of financial status, positive psychology emphasizes leveraging client strengths and sense of accomplishment to enhance a more holistic sense of wellness (Seligman 2011). Over the years, social psychology has studied message framing by evaluating whether a message highlights the positive benefits of taking action or the negative consequences of not taking action (Rothman & Salovey 1997). For example, ‘exercising regularly will boost your energy, and build a healthy lifestyle’ versus ‘not exercising regularly will make you feel tired and put you at higher risk for health problems’. In a financial context, ‘if you start saving money now for retirement, you will be able to enjoy later in life’, versus ‘If you don’t start saving now for retirement, you won’t be able to enjoy life later’. Although both messages deliver the same information about exercise and retirement planning, research suggests that the positively framed message may be more persuasive. According to Salovey and Williams-Piehota (2004), factually equivalent information can be shared in a way that makes people see it as a gain or a loss. This has also been shown to be particularly effective when influencing motivation. For instance, when goals are framed with an emphasis on achieving positive outcomes, individuals tend to experience increased motivation and positive emotions (Roney
Positive framing also plays a role in how individuals see themselves. In a study of newly married couples, Cast and Cantwell (2007) found that both positive and negative feedback from significant others is influential in how people see themselves. Positively framed messages about the benefits of interventions (e.g. ‘This treatment has a 90% survival rate’ versus ‘This treatment has a 10% mortality rate’) tends to be associated with more favourable attitudes towards treatment options and increased patient engagement in the medical field (Edwards
Neuroimaging research suggests that positively framed feedback activates the brain’s reward centers associated with motivation and goal-directed behaviour (Sharot
While the evidence for positive framing is generally strong across various fields of study, overly optimistic framing may lead to unrealistic expectations or lead individuals to avoid confronting negative realities, especially where negative outcomes or risks must be communicated (Edwards
Data were collected as part of a pilot study for a larger longitudinal study of the impact of financial planning on client/consumer outcomes; as such, this sample was meant to test outcome measures associated with the use of financial planning services. The data were collected through an Alchemer sample of consumers (
Because of the nature of the pilot study, limited demographic data are available. Respondents were asked to indicate their year of birth. To calculate age, the year of birth was subtracted from the year of data collection (i.e. 2024). Household income was captured by asking respondents to report their actual household income to their best estimate. Assets were collected by respondent’s response to the following question: What is the total value of your household’s investable assets? Investable assets include all liquid financial assets that are, or could be invested (e.g. bank account balances, retirement accounts, trusts, etc.). Investable assets do not include businesses, real estate or other property. Options included the seven categories of (a) less than $50,000; (b) $50,000 to $99,999; (c) $100,000 to $249,999; (d) $250,000 to $499,999; (e) $500,000 to $749,999; (f) $750,000 to $999,999; and (g) $1,000,000 or more.
According to previous research, an essential indicator of individuals’ ability to make financial decisions is their level of financial literacy. Financial literacy encompasses not only knowledge and understanding of financial concepts, but also the ability to apply this knowledge. Thus, financial literacy includes both knowledge and behaviour (Lusardi 2019). In this study, financial literacy was measured using a set of five questions covering fundamental economic and financial concepts. These questions, known as the Big Five, expand upon the Big Three (Lusardi & Mitchell 2011; Lusardi & Mitchell 2014) and are among the most used to measure financial literacy. The Big Five consists of multiple-choice questions assessing aptitude in simple interest calculations (related to savings accounts and inflation) and understanding the relationship between interest rates and bond prices. Additionally, the Big Five include two true/false questions that test knowledge of the relationship between mortgage length and total interest paid and the concept of risk diversification. The questions are presented in Table 2. They each measure a distinct area of personal finances and are not meant to be used as a “scale” with a computed Cronbach’s alpha. The five items were totaled, however, to create a range of accumulated knowledge from 0 to 5 correct responses.
Big 5 Financial Literacy Scale
Suppose you have $100 in a savings account earning 2% interest a year. After five years, how much would you have? Exactly $102, Less than $102, More than $102 | More than $102 |
Imagine that the interest rate on your savings account is 1% a year and inflation is 2% a year. After one year, would the money in the account buy more than it does today, exactly the same or less than today? | Less than today |
If interest rates rise, what will typically happen to bond prices? Rise, fall, stay the same, or is there no relationship? | Fall |
True or false: A 15-year mortgage typically requires higher monthly payments than a 30-year mortgage but the total interest over the life of the loan will be less. | True |
True or false: Buying a single company’s stock usually provides a safer return than a stock mutual fund. | False |
To test the framing effect, a peer comparison option was presented in one of two ways: (a) ‘Compared to my peers, I am better off financially’ versus (b) ‘Compared to my peers, I am worse off financially’. Respondents were asked to respond on a five-point scale of 1 = Strongly agree; 2 = Agree; 2.5 = Neither agree nor disagree; 3 = Disagree; and 4 = Strongly disagree. Scores were reverse coded when presented with the ‘worse off’ scenario and merged into a new single item variable to test the framing effect. To test the framing effect, the new combined variable for ‘framing’ had the categories of 1 if shown the ‘better off’ condition and 0 if shown the ‘worse off’ condition. The questions that immediately followed the framing question are detailed in the Appendix and displayed in the order in which they appeared to respondents.
To explore these wellness characteristics, respondents were asked a series of feelings-based questions and resource management questions. Items were selected to determine how individuals perceive both their happiness and financial stability, capturing emotional dimensions of well-being and objective financial security.
Firstly, respondents were asked to assess their own happiness with the statement:
To evaluate respondents’ resource management within financial wellness, they were asked,
Table 3 summarizes the known demographic characteristics of the sample. The sample surveyed is generally middle-aged (M = 43 years of age) and has income slightly above the national median. Based on a standard five-item financial literacy knowledge battery, the typical respondent could correctly answer about half of the questions, which is similar to other surveys of respondents in the United States. Fully one-third of respondents had less than $50,000 in investable assets, although around one in five has more than $500,000 in assets. Overall, the sample is a reasonable proxy for populations engaging in some level of formal or informal financial planning.
Available Descriptive Statistics
Age | 712 | 43.28 | 13.40 | 25-65 |
Household Income | 624 | $138,737 | $587,829 | $0−2,000,000 |
Financial Literacy Score (/5) | 729 | 2.50 | 1.45 | 0–5 |
Assets | Frequency (%) | |||
Less than $50,000 | 232 | 30 | ||
$50,000 to $99,999 | 178 | 23 | ||
$100,000 to $249,999 | 126 | 16 | ||
$250,000 to $499,999 | 72 | 9 | ||
$500,000 to $749,999 | 58 | 8 | ||
$750,000 to $999,999 | 52 | 7 | ||
$1,000,000 or more | 50 | 7 |
Respondents who were shown the ‘better off’ compared to peers framing reported their situation more favourably (M = 3.41) than those who were shown the ‘worse off’ framing (M = 3.06;
I use money to create joy and happiness in my life. | 1.99 (0.64) | 2.08 (0.64) | −2.07(874)* |
I frequently have conflict with family members about money. | 2.75 (0.94) | 2.75 (0.87) | 0.02(869) n.s. |
When you think about your personal or household financial situation, which of the following words describes how you feel? (% Responding ‘Yes’ to ‘Prepared’) | 0.66 (0.47) | 0.73 (0.45) | −2.05(859)* |
If I die tomorrow, my family will be financially okay. | 2.06 (0.81) | 2.12 (0.82) | −0.87 (874) n.s. |
If I get sick and unable to work for a long time, my family will be financially okay. | 2.14 (0.82) | 2.18 (0.83) | −0.67 (875) n.s. |
Overall, which one of the following best describes how well you are managing financially these days? | 2.65 (0.92) | 2.78 (0.91) | −2.04(846)* |
Mean Age | 43.43 (12.89) | 43.13 (13.91) | n.s. |
Mean Income | $134,953 (584,886) | $142,600 (587,829) | n.s. |
Mean Financial Literacy (/5) | 2.49 (1.44) | 2.51 (1.47) | n.s. |
Mean Asset Group (/5) | 2.66 (1.44) | 2.53 (1.37) | n.s. |
Respondents shown the ‘better off’ option reported a statistically higher mean score for “Joy” compared to respondents shown the ‘worse off’ option (M = 2.08 v. 1.99;
There were no statistically significant differences on the two resource management items related to family members being financial okay upon the respondent’s death or disability. Though respondents shown the ‘better off’ option had a more favourable opinion of their overall financial situation compared to those shown the “worse off” option (M = 2.78 v. 2.65;
It is common for people to evaluate their financial situation relative to others, often using peer comparisons as a benchmark for financial success or well-being. This study used a very simple yet effective framing question, prompting respondents to consider if they were better or worse off financially than their peers. Despite its simplicity, this framing resulted in statistically significant differences in how respondents evaluated their financial lives.
In general, the findings align with prospect theory (Kahneman & Tversky 1979), which suggests that people assess their financial position relative to a reference point rather than in absolute terms. When peer comparisons are framed positively, individuals may feel more optimistic and in control of their financial situation, whereas negative framing may exacerbate feelings of financial stress or other less favourable associations. The results showed evidence for this in that respondents who were presented with positive framing of their financial situation compared to their peers reported that they used money to create joy and happiness in their lives, felt financially prepared and felt like they were managing well financially at higher rates than those presented with negative framing (i.e. being worse off than their peers financially). There were no differences in the framing effect for how often respondents reported conflict with their family about money or how well their family was prepared for death or disability of the respondent. This is not surprising, given that these are more quantitative indicators of one’s financial situation versus their perception of or feelings about their situation.
The findings support the hypothesis that the framing of one’s financial condition has a measurable effect on indicators of financial wellness. This phenomenon may be important for research and practice and merits more extensive research and testing. Based on this study, using more positive framing with clients is likely to encourage more favourable attitudes.
The generalizability of the results should be approached with caution due to the absence of extensive demographic data and the inherent bias associated with self-reported financial behaviours. Other limitations of the data include the inherent issues associated with self-reported data and the possibility for inaccurately reported information. Longitudinal data would be useful in ascertaining how long the effect of positive framing holds for clients’ perception of their financial situation.
When financial planners use positive framing techniques, they can help clients build their financial wellness. The way information is presented influences how individuals interpret and respond to it (Seligman 2011). Positive framing can help clients feel joy, happiness, contentment and prepared to deal with their financial situation. The advantages of positive framing are supported by findings in psychology (Roney
While positive framing can be highly effective, it is important to avoid overlooking potential risks or negative outcomes (Edwards
A phrase often used in the authors’ professional work is,