Accesso libero

Stimulus-response binding is not a gradually learned association between specific stimuli and their responses: Evidence from a teenage bilingual population

INFORMAZIONI SU QUESTO ARTICOLO

Cita

Cock, J. J., Berry, D. C., & Buchner, A. (2002). Negative priming and sequence learning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 14(1), 27–48. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440042000151 Search in Google Scholar

Constantinidis, C., & Luna, B. (2019). Neural substrates of inhibitory control maturation in adolescence. Trends in Neurosciences, 42(9), 604–616. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2019.07.004 Search in Google Scholar

Christie, J. J., & Klein, R. M. (2008). On finding negative priming from distractors. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(4), 866–873. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.15.4.866 Search in Google Scholar

Dalrymple-Alford, E. C., & Budayr, B. (1966). Examination of some aspects of the stroop color-word test. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 23(3_suppl), 1211–1214. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1966.23.3f.12 Search in Google Scholar

DeSchepper, B. & Treisman, A. (1996). Visual memory for novel shapes: Implicit coding without attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22(1), 27–47. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.22.1.27 Search in Google Scholar

Fox, E. (1994). Interference and negative priming from ignored distractors: The role of selection difficulty. Perception and Psychophysics, 56(5), 565–574. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206952 Search in Google Scholar

Frings, C., Schneider, K. K., & Fox, E. (2015). The negative priming paradigm: An update and implications for selective attention. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(6), 1577–1597. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0841-4 Search in Google Scholar

Frings, C., Wentura, D., & Wühr, P. (2012). On the fate of distractor representations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(3), 570–575. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027781 Search in Google Scholar

Gamboz, N., Russo, R., & Fox, E. (2000). Target selection difficulty, negative priming, and aging. Psychology and Aging, 15(3), 542–550. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.15.3.542 Search in Google Scholar

Grison, S., & Strayer, D. L. (2001). Negative priming and perceptual fluency: More than 142 what meets the eye. Perception and Psychophysics, 63(6), 1063–1071. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194524 Search in Google Scholar

Henson, R. N., Eckstein, D., Waszak, F., Frings, C., & Horner, A. J. (2014). Stimulus–response bindings in priming. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(7), 376–384. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.03.004 Search in Google Scholar

Houghton, G. & Tipper, S. P. (1994). A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language. In D. Dagenbach & T. H. Carr (Eds.). Inhibitory processes in attention, memory and language (pp. 55–112). Academic Press. Search in Google Scholar

Houghton, G., Tipper, S. P., Weaver, B., & Shore, D. I. (1996). Inhibition and interference in selective attention: Some tests of a neural network model. Visual Cognition, 3, 119–164. https://doi.org/10.1080/713756733 Search in Google Scholar

Kotey, P. A. (2007). Hippocrene concise dictionary: Twi-English, English-Twi. Third printing. Hippocrene Books. Search in Google Scholar

Kramer, A. F., & Strayer, D. L. (2001). Influence of stimulus repetition on negative priming. Psychology and Aging, 16(4), 580–587. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.16.4.580 Search in Google Scholar

Langley, L. K., Overmier, J. B., Knopman, D. S., & Prod’Homme, M. M. (1998). Inhibition and habituation: Preserved mechanisms of attentional selection in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 12, 353–366. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.12.3.353 Search in Google Scholar

MacLeod, C. M., Chiappe, D. L. & Fox, E. (2002). The crucial roles of stimulus matching and stimulus identity in negative priming. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(3), 521–52. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196308 Search in Google Scholar

Malley, G. B., & Strayer, D. L. (1995). Effect of stimulus repetition on positive and negative identity priming. Perception & Psychophysics, 57(5), 657–667. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03213271 Search in Google Scholar

Manyah, K. A. (2011). Nasal-oral contrast of short and long vowels in Twi: An acoustic study. In E. G. Bokamba, R. K. Shosted, & T. Ayalew (Eds.), Selected proceedings of the 40th Annual Conference on African Linguistics (pp. 95–104). Cascadilla Proceedings Project. Search in Google Scholar

Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). The language experience and proficiency questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(4), 940–967. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/067) Search in Google Scholar

Mayr, S. & Buchner, A. (2007). Negative priming as a memory phenomenon: A review of 20 years of negative priming research. Journal of Psychology, 215(1), 35–51. https://doi.org/10.1027/0044-3409.215.1.35 Search in Google Scholar

McLennan, K. S., Neumann, E. & Russell, P. N. (2019). Positive and negative priming differences between short-term and long-term identity coding of word-specific attentional priorities. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 81, 1426–1441. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-018-01661-9 Search in Google Scholar

Neill, W. T. (1997). Episodic retrieval in negative priming and repetition priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 23, 1291–1305. Search in Google Scholar

Neill, W. T., & Valdes, L. A. (1992). Persistence of negative priming: Steady state or decay? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 565–576. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.18.3.565 Search in Google Scholar

Neumann, E., & DeSchepper, B. G. (1991). Costs and benefits of target activation and distractor inhibition in selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 1136–1145. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.17.6.1136 Search in Google Scholar

Neumann, E., McCloskey, M. S., & Felio, A. C. (1999). Cross-language positive priming disappears, negative priming does not: Evidence for two sources of selective inhibition. Memory & Cognition, 27(6), 1051–1063. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03201234 Search in Google Scholar

Nkrumah, I. K., & Neumann, E. (2018). Cross-language negative priming remains intact, while positive priming disappears: Evidence for two sources of selective inhibition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 30(3), 361–384. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2017.1417311 Search in Google Scholar

Ortells, J. J. & Tudela, P. (1996). Positive and negative semantic priming of attended and unattended parafoveal words in a lexical decision task. Acta Psychologica, 94(2), 209–226. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(95)00045-3 Search in Google Scholar

Pritchard, V. E., & Neumann, E. (2009). Avoiding the potential pitfalls of using negative priming tasks in developmental studies: Assessing inhibitory control in children, adolescents and adults. Developmental Psychology, 45(1), 272–283. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014168 Search in Google Scholar

Pritchard, V. E., & Neumann, E. (2011). Classic Stroop negative priming effects for children and adults diverge with less-conflicting and nonconflicting conditions. The American Journal of Psychology, 124(4), 405–419. https://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.124.4.0405 Search in Google Scholar

Richards, A. (1999). The effects of cueing target location and response mode on interference and negative priming using a visual selection paradigm. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 52(2), 449–463. https://doi.org/10.1080/713755825 Search in Google Scholar

Schooler, C., Neumann, E., Caplan, L. J., & Roberts, B. R. (1997). Continued inhibitory capacity throughout adulthood: Conceptual negative priming in younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 12(4), 667–674. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.12.4.667 Search in Google Scholar

Sprondel, V., Kipp, K. H., & Mecklinger, A. (2011). Developmental changes in item and source memory: Evidence from an ERP recognition memory study with children, adolescents, and adults. Child Development, 82(6), 1638–1953. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01642.x Search in Google Scholar

Strayer, D. L., & Grison, S. (1999). Negative identity priming is contingent on stimulus repetition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25(1), 24–38. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.25.1.24 Search in Google Scholar

Tipper, S. P. (1985). The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 37, 571–590. https://doi.org/10.1080/14640748508400920 Search in Google Scholar

Tipper, S. P. (2001). Does negative priming reflect inhibitory mechanisms? A review and integration of conflicting views. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology, 54(2), 321–343. https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980042000183 Search in Google Scholar

Treisman, A., & Gormican, S. (1988). Feature analysis in early vision: Evidence from search asymmetries. Psychological Review, 95(1), 15–48. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.95.1.15 Search in Google Scholar

eISSN:
2083-8506
Lingua:
Inglese
Frequenza di pubblicazione:
Volume Open
Argomenti della rivista:
Social Sciences, Psychology, Applied Psychology