Accesso libero

Developing Theory of Mind Twenty-Five Years After the Publication of “Z Badań Nad Kompetencją Komunikacyjną Dziecka” (Edited by B. Bokus and M.Haman)

   | 11 lug 2019
INFORMAZIONI SU QUESTO ARTICOLO

Cita

Apperly, I. (2013). Can theory of mind grow up? Mindreading in adults, and its implications for the development and neuroscience of mindreading. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H. & Lombardo, M. (Eds.) Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental social neuroscience (pp. 72–92). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Apperly, I. A. & Butterfill, S. A. (2009). Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychological review, 116(4), 953.10.1037/a0016923Search in Google Scholar

Astington, J. W. (1993). The child’s discovery of the mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Baillargeon, R., Buttelmann, D., & Southgate, V. (2018). Invited commentary: Interpreting failed replications of early false-belief findings: Methodological and theoretical considerations. Cognitive Development, 46, 112–124.10.1016/j.cogdev.2018.06.001Search in Google Scholar

Baker, S. T., Leslie, A. M., Gallistel, C. R., & Hood, B. M. (2016). Bayesian change-point analysis reveals developmental change in a classic theory of mind task. Cognitive psychology, 91, 124–149.10.1016/j.cogpsych.2016.08.001Search in Google Scholar

Bardi, L., Desmet, C., Nijhof, A., Wiersema, J. R., & Brass, M. (2016). Brain activation for spontaneous and explicit false belief tasks overlaps: new fMRI evidence on belief processing and violation of expectation. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 12(3), 391–400.10.1093/scan/nsw143Search in Google Scholar

Bardi, L., Six, P., & Brass, M. (2017). Repetitive TMS of the temporo-parietal junction disrupts participant’s expectations in a spontaneous Theory of Mind task. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 12(11), 1775–1782.10.1093/scan/nsx109Search in Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). The autistic child’s theory of mind: A case of specific developmental delay. Journal of child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30(2), 285–297.10.1111/j.1469-7610.1989.tb00241.xSearch in Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”?. Cognition, 21(1), 37–46.10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8Search in Google Scholar

Benson, J. E., Sabbagh, M. A., Carlson, S. M., & Zelazo, P. D. (2013). Individual differences in executive functioning predict preschoolers’ improvement from theory-of-mind training. Developmental Psychology, 49(9), 1615.10.1037/a0031056Search in Google Scholar

Birch, S. A., & Bloom, P. (2003). Children are cursed: An asymmetric bias in mental-state attribution. Psychological Science, 14(3), 283–286.10.1111/1467-9280.03436Search in Google Scholar

Birch, S. A., & Bloom, P. (2007). The curse of knowledge in reasoning about false-beliefs. Psychological Science, 18(5), 382–386.10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01909.xSearch in Google Scholar

Biro, S., Csibra, G., Koos, O, & Gergely, G. (1998). Understanding rational action in infancy. Psychology of Language and Communication, 1(2), 29–38. Bloom, P., & German, T. P. (2000). Two reasons to abandon the false-belief task as a test of theory of mind. Cognition, 77(1), B25–B31.Search in Google Scholar

Buttelmann, D., Buttelmann, F., Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Great apes distinguish true from false-beliefs in an interactive helping task. PLoS one, 12(4), e0173793.10.1371/journal.pone.0173793Search in Google Scholar

Buttelmann, D., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Eighteen-month-old infants show false-belief understanding in an active helping paradigm. Cognition, 112(2), 337–342.10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.006Search in Google Scholar

Buttelmann, D., Over, H., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Eighteen-month-olds understand false-beliefs in an unexpected-contents task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 119, 120–126.10.1016/j.jecp.2013.10.002Search in Google Scholar

Buttelmann, F., Suhrke, J., & Buttelmann, D. (2015). What you get is what you believe: Eighteen-month-olds demonstrate belief understanding in an unexpected-identity task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 131, 94–103.10.1016/j.jecp.2014.11.009Search in Google Scholar

Carruthers, P. (2017). Mindreading in adults: evaluating two-systems views. Synthese, 194(3), 673–688.10.1007/s11229-015-0792-3Search in Google Scholar

Carter, C. S., & Van Veen, V. (2007). Anterior cingulate cortex and conflict detection: an update of theory and data. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(4), 367-379.10.3758/CABN.7.4.367Search in Google Scholar

Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A. (1997). Infants with autism: An investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention, and imitation. Developmental Psychology, 33(5), 781.10.1037/0012-1649.33.5.781Search in Google Scholar

Clements, W. A., & Perner, J. (1994). Implicit understanding of belief. Cognitive Development, 9(4), 377–395.10.1016/0885-2014(94)90012-4Search in Google Scholar

Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (1998). The teleological origins of mentalistic action explanations: A developmental hypothesis. Developmental Science, 1(2), 255–259.10.1111/1467-7687.00039Search in Google Scholar

Dennett, D. C. (1978). Beliefs about beliefs [P&W, SR&B]. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 568–570.10.1017/S0140525X00076664Search in Google Scholar

de Villiers, J. G., & de Villiers, P. A. (2000). Linguistic determinism and the understanding of false. Children’s Reasoning and the Mind (Eds.), P. Mitchell and K. Riggs (Hove: Psychology Press), 191–228.Search in Google Scholar

de Villiers, J. G., & Pyers, J. E. (2002). Complements to cognition: A longitudinal study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-understanding. Cognitive Development, 17(1), 1037–1060.10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00073-4Search in Google Scholar

Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2014). Relations between false-belief understanding and executive function in early childhood: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 85(5), 1777–1794.10.1111/cdev.12237Search in Google Scholar

Drayton, L. A., & Santos, L. R. (2018). What do monkeys know about others’ knowledge?. Cognition, 170, 201–208.10.1016/j.cognition.2017.10.004Search in Google Scholar

Farrar, M. J., Benigno, J. P., Tompkins, V., & Gage, N. A. (2017). Are there different pathways to explicit false-belief understanding? General language and complementation in typical and atypical children. Cognitive Development, 43, 49–66.10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.02.005Search in Google Scholar

Flavell, J. H. (1992). Perspectives on perspective-taking. Piaget’s theory: Prospects and possibilities. (pp. 107–139). Hillsdale, NJ: ErlbaumSearch in Google Scholar

Flavell, J. H. (1993). The development of children’s understanding of false-belief and the appearance-reality distinction. International Journal of Psychology, 28(5), 595–604.10.1080/00207599308246944Search in Google Scholar

Fletcher, P. C., Happe, F., Frith, U., Baker, S. C., Dolan, R. J., Frackowiak, R. S., & Frith, C. D. (1995). Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension. Cognition, 57(2), 109–128.10.1016/0010-0277(95)00692-RSearch in Google Scholar

Gallagher, H. L., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(2), 77–83.10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00025-6Search in Google Scholar

Gallagher, S., & Povinelli, D. J. (2012). Enactive and behavioral abstraction accounts of social understanding in chimpanzees, infants, and adults. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 3(1), 145–169.10.1007/s13164-012-0093-4Search in Google Scholar

Gergely, G., Nádasdy, Z., Csibra, G., & Bíró, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56(2), 165–193.10.1016/0010-0277(95)00661-HSearch in Google Scholar

Grosse Wiesmann, C., Friederici, A. D., Disla, D., Steinbeis, N., & Singer, T. (2018). Longitudinal evidence for 4-year-olds’ but not 2-and 3-year-olds’ false-belief-related action anticipation. Cognitive Development, 46, 58–68.10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.007Search in Google Scholar

Grosse Wiesmann, C., Friederici, A. D., Singer, T., & Steinbeis, N. (2017). Implicit and explicit false-belief development in preschool children. Developmental Science, 20(5).Search in Google Scholar

Grosse Wiesmann, C., Schreiber, J., Singer, T., Steinbeis, N., & Friederici, A. D. (2017). White matter maturation is associated with the emergence of theory of mind in early childhood. Nature Communications, 8, 14692.Search in Google Scholar

Gut, A., Haman, M. & Gorbaniuk, O. (under review) The development of understanding opacity in preschoolers: A transition from a coarse- to fine-grained understanding of beliefs.Search in Google Scholar

Gweon, H., Dodell-Feder, D., Bedny, M., & Saxe, R. (2012). Theory of mind performance in children correlates with functional specialization of a brain region for thinking about thoughts. Child development, 83(6), 1853–1868.10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01829.xSearch in Google Scholar

Helming, K. A., Strickland, B., & Jacob, P. (2014). Making sense of early false-belief understanding. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(4), 167–170.10.1016/j.tics.2014.01.005Search in Google Scholar

Helming, K. A., Strickland, B., & Jacob, P. (2016). Solving the puzzle about early belief-ascription. Mind & Language, 31(4), 438–469.10.1111/mila.12114Search in Google Scholar

Henry, J. D., Phillips, L. H., Ruffman, T., & Bailey, P. E. (2013). A meta-analytic review of age differences in theory of mind. Psychology and Aging, 28(3), 826.10.1037/a0030677Search in Google Scholar

Heyes, C. (2014a). False-belief in infancy: a fresh look. Developmental Science, 17(5), 647–659.10.1111/desc.12148Search in Google Scholar

Heyes, C. (2014b). Submentalizing: I am not really reading your mind. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(2), 131–143.10.1177/1745691613518076Search in Google Scholar

Hoehl, S., Wiese, L., & Striano, T. (2008). Young infants’ neural processing of objects is affected by eye gaze direction and emotional expression. PLoS One, 3(6), e2389.10.1371/journal.pone.0002389Search in Google Scholar

Hood, B. M., Willen, J. D., & Driver, J. (1998). Adult’s eyes trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants. Psychological Science, 9(2), 131–134.10.1111/1467-9280.00024Search in Google Scholar

Hughes, C., & Leekam, S. (2004). What are the links between theory of mind and social relations? Review, reflections and new directions for studies of typical and atypical development. Social Development, 13(4), 590–619.10.1111/j.1467-9507.2004.00285.xSearch in Google Scholar

Hyde, D. C., Aparicio Betancourt, M., & Simon, C. E. (2015). Human temporal- parietal junction spontaneously tracks others' beliefs: A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. Human Brain Mapping, 36(12), 4831–4846.10.1002/hbm.22953Search in Google Scholar

Hyde, D. C., Simon, C. E., Ting, F., & Nikolaeva, J. I. (2018). Functional Organization of the Temporal–Parietal Junction for Theory of Mind in Preverbal Infants: A Near-Infrared Spectroscopy Study. Journal of Neuroscience, 38(18), 4264–4274.10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0264-17.2018Search in Google Scholar

Jacoby, N., Bruneau, E., Koster-Hale, J., & Saxe, R. (2016). Localizing pain matrix and theory of mind networks with both verbal and non-verbal stimuli. NeuroImage, 126, 39–48.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.025Search in Google Scholar

Jenkins, J. M., & Astington, J. W. (2000). Theory of mind and social behavior: Causal models tested in a longitudinal study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly (1982-), 203–220.Search in Google Scholar

Kampis, D., Fogd, D., & Kovács, Á. M. (2017). Nonverbal components of Theory of Mind in typical and atypical development. Infant Behavior and Development, 48, 54–62.10.1016/j.infbeh.2016.11.001Search in Google Scholar

Kovács, Á. M., Kühn, S., Gergely, G., Csibra, G., & Brass, M. (2014). Are all beliefs equal? Implicit belief attributions recruiting core brain regions of theory of mind. PloS one, 9(9), e106558.10.1371/journal.pone.0106558Search in Google Scholar

Kovács, Á. M., Téglás, E., & Endress, A. D. (2010). The social sense: Susceptibility to others’ beliefs in human infants and adults. Science, 330(6012), 1830–1834.Search in Google Scholar

El Kaddouri, R., Bardi, L., De Bremaeker, D., Brass, M., & Wiersema, J. R. (2019). Measuring spontaneous mentalizing with a ball detection task: putting the attention-check hypothesis by Phillips and colleagues (2015) to the test. Psychological Research, 1–9.Search in Google Scholar

Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false-beliefs. Science, 354(6308), 110–114.Search in Google Scholar

Krupenye, C., Kano, F., Hirata, S., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2017). A test of the submentalizing hypothesis: Apes’ performance in a false-belief task inanimate control. Communicative & Integrative Biology, 10(4), e1343771.10.1080/19420889.2017.1343771Search in Google Scholar

Kuhn, D. (2000). Theory of mind, metacognition, and reasoning: A life-span perspective. In P. Mitchell & K. J. Riggs (Eds.) Children’s reasoning and the mind (pp. 301–326). Hove: Psychology Press.Search in Google Scholar

Kulke, L., & Rakoczy, H. (2018). Implicit theory of mind – An overview of current replications and non-replications. Data in Brief, 16, 101–104.10.1016/j.dib.2017.11.016Search in Google Scholar

Kulke, L., Reiß, M., Krist, H., & Rakoczy, H. (2018). How robust are anticipatory looking measures of theory of mind? Replication attempts across the life span. Cognitive Development, 46, 97–111.10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.09.001Search in Google Scholar

Kulke, L., von Duhn, B., Schneider, D., & Rakoczy, H. (2018). Is implicit theory of mind a real and robust phenomenon? Results from a systematic replication study. Psychological Science, 0956797617747090.10.1177/0956797617747090Search in Google Scholar

Lackner, C., Sabbagh, M. A., Hallinan, E., Liu, X., & Holden, J. J. (2012). Dopamine receptor D4 gene variation predicts preschoolers’ developing theory of mind. Developmental Science, 15(2), 272–280.10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01124.xSearch in Google Scholar

Lackner, C. L., Bowman, L. C., & Sabbagh, M. A. (2010). Dopaminergic functioning and preschoolers’ theory of mind. Neuropsychologia, 48(6), 1767–1774.10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.02.027Search in Google Scholar

Leslie, A. M., Xu, F., Tremoulet, P. D., & Scholl, B. J. (1998). Indexing and the object concept: developingwhat’andwhere’systems. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(1), 10–18.10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01113-3Search in Google Scholar

Lillard, A. (1998). Ethnopsychologies: cultural variations in theories of mind. Psychological Bulletin, 123(1), 3.10.1037/0033-2909.123.1.3Search in Google Scholar

Low, J. (2010). Preschoolers’ implicit and explicit false-belief understanding: relations with complex syntactical mastery. Child Development, 81(2), 597–61510.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01418.xSearch in Google Scholar

Low, J., Apperly, I. A., Butterfill, S. A., & Rakoczy, H. (2016). Cognitive architecture of reasoning in children and adults: A primer on the two-systems account. Child Development Perspectives, 10(3), 184-189.10.1111/cdep.12183Search in Google Scholar

Mars, R. B., Neubert, F. X., Noonan, M. P., Sallet, J., Toni, I., & Rushworth, M. F. (2012). On the relationship between the “default mode network” and the “social brain”. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 189.10.3389/fnhum.2012.00189Search in Google Scholar

Meert, G., Wang, J., & Samson, D. (2017). Efficient belief tracking in adults: The role of task instruction, low-level associative processes and dispositional social functioning. Cognition, 168, 91–98.10.1016/j.cognition.2017.06.012Search in Google Scholar

Meltzoff, A. N. (1993). The role of imitation in understanding persons and developing a theory of mind. Understanding other minds: Perspectives from Autism, 335–366.Search in Google Scholar

Moran, J. M., Young, L. L., Saxe, R., Lee, S. M., O’Young, D., Mavros, P. L., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2011). Impaired theory of mind for moral judgment in high-functioning autism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(7), 2688–2692.10.1073/pnas.1011734108Search in Google Scholar

Moses, L. J. (2001). Executive accounts of theory-of-mind development. Child Development, 72(3), 688–690.10.1111/1467-8624.00306Search in Google Scholar

Naughtin, C. K., Horne, K., Schneider, D., Venini, D., York, A., & Dux, P. E. (2017). Do implicit and explicit belief processing share neural substrates? Human Brain Mapping, 38(9), 4760–4772.10.1002/hbm.23700Search in Google Scholar

Nelson, K. (1998). Language in cognitive development: The emergence of the mediated mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Nelson, K., Plesa, D., & Henseler, S. (1998). Children’s theory of mind: An experiential interpretation. Human Development, 41(1), 7–29.10.1159/000022565Search in Google Scholar

Nijhof, A. D., Brass, M., Bardi, L., & Wiersema, J. R. (2016). Measuring mentalizing ability: A within-subject comparison between an explicit and implicit version of a ball detection task. PloS one, 11(10): e0164373. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.016437310.1371/journal.pone.0164373Search in Google Scholar

Nijhof, A. D., Bardi, L., Brass, M., & Wiersema, J. R. (2018). Brain activity for spontaneous and explicit mentalizing in adults with autism spectrum disorder: An fMRI study. NeuroImage: Clinical, 18, 475–484.10.1016/j.nicl.2018.02.016Search in Google Scholar

Oktay-Gür, N., Schulz, A., & Rakoczy, H. (2018). Children exhibit different performance patterns in explicit and implicit theory of mind tasks. Cognition, 173, 60–74.10.1016/j.cognition.2018.01.001Search in Google Scholar

Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false-beliefs? Science, 308(5719), 255–258.Search in Google Scholar

Peterson, C., Slaughter, V., Moore, C., & Wellman, H. M. (2016). Peer social skills and theory of mind in children with autism, deafness, or typical development. Developmental Psychology, 52(1), 46.10.1037/a0039833Search in Google Scholar

Peterson, C. C., Slaughter, V. P., & Paynter, J. (2007). Social maturity and theory of mind in typically developing children and those on the autism spectrum. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(12), 1243–1250.10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01810.xSearch in Google Scholar

Phillips, J., Ong, D. C., Surtees, A. D., Xin, Y., Williams, S., Saxe, R., & Frank, M. C. (2015). A second look at automatic theory of mind: Reconsidering Kovács, Téglás, and Endress (2010). Psychological Science, 26(9), 1353–1367.Search in Google Scholar

Poulin-Dubois, D., & Yott, J. (2018). Probing the depth of infants’ theory of mind: Disunity in performance across paradigms. Developmental Science, 21(4), e12600.10.1111/desc.12600Search in Google Scholar

Premack, D. (1990). The infant’s theory of self-propelled objects. Cognition, 36(1), 1–16.10.1016/0010-0277(90)90051-KSearch in Google Scholar

Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515–526.10.1017/S0140525X00076512Search in Google Scholar

Priewasser, B., Rafetseder, E., Gargitter, C., & Perner, J. (2017). Helping as an early indicator of a theory of mind: Mentalism or teleology?. Cognitive Development, 46, 69–78. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.00210.1016/j.cogdev.2017.08.002Open DOISearch in Google Scholar

Rabinowitz, N. C., Perbet, F., Song, H. F., Zhang, C., Eslami, S. M., & Botvinick, M. (2018). Machine theory of mind. arXiv preprint arXiv:1802.07740.Search in Google Scholar

Richardson, H., Lisandrelli, G., Riobueno-Naylor, A., & Saxe, R. (2018). Development of the social brain from age three to twelve years. Nature Communications, 9(1), 1027.10.1038/s41467-018-03399-2Search in Google Scholar

Rubio-Fernández, P., & Geurts, B. (2013). How to pass the false-belief task before your fourth birthday. Psychological Science, 24(1), 27–33.10.1177/0956797612447819Search in Google Scholar

Rubio-Fernández, P., Jara-Ettinger, J., & Gibson, E. (2017). Can processing demands explain toddlers’ performance in false-belief tasks?. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(19), E3750–E3750.10.1073/pnas.1701286114Search in Google Scholar

Sabbagh, M. A., Bowman, L. C., Evraire, L. E., & Ito, J. (2009). Neurodevelopmental correlates of theory of mind in preschool children. Child Development, 80(4), 1147–1162.10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01322.xSearch in Google Scholar

Samson, D., Apperly, I., Braithwaite, J., Andrews, B., & Bodley Scott, S. (2010). Seeing it their way: Evidence for rapid and involuntary computation of what other people see. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 1255–1266.Search in Google Scholar

Schaafsma, S. M., Pfaff, D. W., Spunt, R. P., & Adolphs, R. (2015). Deconstructing and reconstructing theory of mind. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(2), 65–72.10.1016/j.tics.2014.11.007Search in Google Scholar

Scheeren, A. M., de Rosnay, M., Koot, H. M., & Begeer, S. (2013). Rethinking theory of mind in high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(6), 628–635.10.1111/jcpp.12007Search in Google Scholar

Schick, B., de Villiers, P., de Villiers, J., & Hoffmeister, R. (2007). Language and theory of mind: A study of deaf children. Child Development, 78(2), 376–396.10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01004.xSearch in Google Scholar

Schneider, D., Lam, R., Bayliss, A. P., & Dux, P. E. (2012). Cognitive load disrupts implicit theory-of-mind processing. Psychological Science, 23(8), 842–847.10.1177/0956797612439070Search in Google Scholar

Schneider, D., Slaughter, V. P., Becker, S. I., & Dux, P. E. (2014). Implicit false-belief processing in the human brain. NeuroImage, 101, 268–275.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.014Search in Google Scholar

Schneider, D., Nott, Z. E., & Dux, P. E. (2014). Task instructions and implicit theory of mind. Cognition, 133, 43–47.10.1016/j.cognition.2014.05.016Search in Google Scholar

Schurz, M., Radua, J., Aichhorn, M., Richlan, F., & Perner, J. (2014). Fractionating theory of mind: A meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 42, 9–34.10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.009Search in Google Scholar

Schurz, M., Tholen, M. G., Perner, J., Mars, R. B., & Sallet, J. (2017). Specifying the brain anatomy underlying temporo-parietal junction activations for theory of mind: A review using probabilistic atlases from different imaging modalities. Human Brain Mapping, 38(9), 4788–4805.10.1002/hbm.23675Search in Google Scholar

Schuwerk, T., Vuori, M., & Sodian, B. (2015). Implicit and explicit theory of mind reasoning in autism spectrum disorders: The impact of experience. Autism, 19(4), 459–468.10.1177/1362361314526004Search in Google Scholar

Scott, R. M. (2014). Post hoc versus predictive accounts of children’s theory of mind: A reply to Ruffman. Developmental Review, 34(3), 300–304.10.1016/j.dr.2014.05.001Search in Google Scholar

Scott, R. M., & Baillargeon, R. (2009). Which penguin is this? Attributing false beliefs about object identity at 18 months. Child Development, 80(4), 1172–1196.10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01324.xSearch in Google Scholar

Scott, R. M., & Baillargeon, R. (2017). Early false-belief understanding. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(4), 237–249.10.1016/j.tics.2017.01.012Search in Google Scholar

Shahaeian, A., Peterson, C. C., Slaughter, V., & Wellman, H. M. (2011). Culture and the sequence of steps in theory of mind development. Developmental Psychology, 47(5), 1239.10.1037/a0023899Search in Google Scholar

Slaughter, V. (2015). Theory of mind in infants and young children: a review. Australian Psychologist, 50(3), 169–172.10.1111/ap.12080Search in Google Scholar

Sodian, B. (2016). Is false-belief understanding continuous from infancy to preschool age? In D. Barner & A. S. Baron (Eds.), Core knowledge and conceptual change (p. 301), Oxford: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Sommerville, J. A., Bernstein, D. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2013). Measuring beliefs in centimeters: Private knowledge biases preschoolers’ and adults’ representation of others’ beliefs. Child Development, 84(6), 1846–1854.10.1111/cdev.12110Search in Google Scholar

Southgate, V. (2018). The puzzle of early mentalizing: A unitary theory of infants’ successes and preschoolers’ failures. doi:10.17605/OSF.IO/Y2GHQSearch in Google Scholar

Spreng, R. N., & Grady, C. L. (2010). Patterns of brain activity supporting autobiographical memory, prospection, and theory of mind, and their relationship to the default mode network. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(6), 1112–1123.10.1162/jocn.2009.21282Search in Google Scholar

Stojnić, G. (2017). Can two-year olds understand others’ false-beliefs about object identities? (Doctoral dissertation, Rutgers University-Graduate School-New Brunswick).Search in Google Scholar

Stojnić, G. & Leslie, A. M. (2018). Poster presented at 9th Budapest Conference on Cognitive Development, Budapest, January 2018.Search in Google Scholar

Thoermer, C., Sodian, B., Vuori, M., Perst, H., & Kristen, S. (2012). Continuity from an implicit to an explicit understanding of false-belief from infancy to preschool age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30(1), 172–187.10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02067.xSearch in Google Scholar

Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. Joint Attention: Its Origins and Role in Development, 103–130.Search in Google Scholar

Tomasello, M. (2018). How children come to understand false-beliefs: A shared intentionality account. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(34), 8491–8498.10.1073/pnas.1804761115Search in Google Scholar

Wang, L., & Leslie, A. M. (2016). Is Implicit Theory of Mind the ‘Real Deal’? The Own-Belief/True-Belief Default in Adults and Young Preschoolers. Mind & Language, 31(2), 147–176.10.1111/mila.12099Search in Google Scholar

Wellman, H. M. (2014). Making minds: How theory of mind develops. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199334919.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Wellman, H. M. (2017). The development of theory of mind: Historical reflections. Child Development Perspectives, 11(3), 207–214.10.1111/cdep.12236Search in Google Scholar

Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 75(2), 523–541.10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.xSearch in Google Scholar

Wellman, H. M., & Woolley, J. D. (1990). From simple desires to ordinary beliefs: The early development of everyday psychology. Cognition, 35, 245–275.10.1016/0010-0277(90)90024-ESearch in Google Scholar

Wellman, H. M., Fang, F., & Peterson, C. C. (2011). Sequential progressions in a theory-of-mind scale: Longitudinal perspectives. Child Development, 82(3), 780–792.10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01583.xSearch in Google Scholar

Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103–128.10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5Search in Google Scholar

Yott, J., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (2016). Are infants’ theory-of-mind abilities well integrated? Implicit understanding of intentions, desires, and beliefs. Journal of Cognition and Development, 17(5), 683–698.10.1080/15248372.2015.1086771Search in Google Scholar

Zaitchik, D. (1990). When representations conflict with reality: The preschooler’s problem with false-beliefs and “false” photographs. Cognition, 35(1), 41–68.10.1016/0010-0277(90)90036-JSearch in Google Scholar

Zelazo, P. D., Carlson, S. M., & Kesek, A. (2008). The development of executive function in childhood. In C. A. Nelson & M. Luciana (Eds.), Developmental cognitive neuroscience. Handbook of developmental cognitive neuroscience (pp. 553–574). Cambridge, MA, US: MIT PresSearch in Google Scholar

eISSN:
2083-8506
Lingua:
Inglese
Frequenza di pubblicazione:
Volume Open
Argomenti della rivista:
Social Sciences, Psychology, Applied Psychology