Accesso libero

The relationship between behavioral problems and screen time in children during COVID-19 school closures in Japan

INFORMAZIONI SU QUESTO ARTICOLO

Cita

Introduction

Pediatricians report that patients’ physical and psychological complaints increase after long periods of school closures in Japan, such as summer vacations. It has been reported that the number of children who commit suicide is greatest in September in Japan (1); therefore, the Japanese government has alerted pediatricians and parents to pay attention to subtle changes in children when they are due to return to school. Hence, long school closures seem to affect children’s physical and psychological status.

Owing to the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19), schools were unexpectedly and abruptly closed in many countries as part of physical distancing policy. The Japanese government ordered the closure of all schools on February 27, 2020, and a state of emergency was declared nationwide on April 16. The period of the school closure varied by region; however, people were requested to remain at home and have restricted contact with non-family members. Given this unusual school closure with these severe restrictions, children’s lives were more severely affected than by conventional annual closures (2). In Saga, Japan, the state of emergency was lifted on May 14, 2020; subsequently, schools gradually reopened, and all schools in this area were open as usual by June 1, 2020.

These unusual conditions prompted us to investigate how this school closure affected schoolchildren. Studies have revealed that COVID-19 school closures exerted profound negative impacts on schoolchildren physically (3) and psychologically (4, 5, 6, 7); however, thus far, no extensive study has been conducted of the precise mechanisms affecting children’s mental health. Therefore, this work was driven by the following research questions: 1) How did the incidence of physical and mental problems change during and after the COVID-19 school closure in Japan? 2) What factors were associated with children experiencing these problems? We hypothesized that there would be a relationship between increased screen time and the incidence of such problems. We also assessed the connection between sleep habits and children’s problems.

Methods
Participants

This was a cross-sectional study conducted in July 2020, which enrolled students from all four public elementary schools in Miyaki-Machi, a suburban town in Saga prefecture, Japan. Parents received a letter describing the study and a questionnaire to be returned to the school by July 30 after completion. Participants were offered no financial incentive. The Regional Research Ethics Committee approved the study (approval number: 02-19) before its commencement.

Measures

All data were anonymously reported by parents or guardians. Five main types of questions were created for this study. Specifically, parents and guardians were asked to identify the characteristics of the participants such as gender and grades, time spent watching TV and/or videos and playing video games, sleep habits (wakeup time and bedtime) during school closure and after school reopened, and behavior related questions, which addressed behavioral problems (irritability, inability to stop playing video games, frequent fights with siblings, excessive dependence on parents, and refusal to sleep independently) during school closure and after school reopened (using binary responses). Total screen time was defined as the sum of the time spent watching TV and/or videos and playing video games. Data on bodyweight change before and after school reopened were also collected (by binary response).

Statistical analyses

All statistical analyses were performed using JMP® Pro version 14 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA). Demographic characteristics were expressed as means and standard deviations for continuous variables and as frequencies and percentages for categorical variables. Chi-squared and Wilcoxon rank sum tests were used to compare differences between groups. Logistic regression analyses were performed to calculate the adjusted odds ratio (OR) with 95% confidence interval (CI) for evaluating the relationships between exhibiting behavioral problems or experiencing bodyweight gain and screen time (total screen time, time spent watching TV and/or videos and playing video games during school closure), respectively. Furthermore, we also analyzed the relationship between exhibiting behavioral problems and experiencing bodyweight gain during school closure. Confounding variables of gender and grades were adjusted for logistic regression analyses. Statistical significance was defined at the .05 level.

Results
Sample characteristics

Figure 1 shows the sample characteristics. A total of 1,290 schoolchildren were enrolled in this study. The response rate was 92.6%. Of the students enrolled, 235 were excluded because of incomplete responses. Overall, 959 children were assessed in this study. In Japan, students in Grade 1 are typically 6 to 7 years old.

Figure 1

Changes in children with behavioral problems

During school closure, 50.3% of children exhibited some problematic behaviors; percentage decreased to 38.0% after schools reopened (-12.3%, p < .001; Table 1). Neither gender nor grades were significantly associated with exhibiting behavioral problems during school closure (Table 2).

Frequency of children with behavioral problems during school closures and after schools reopened

During school closures After schools reopened During vs. after
n (%) n (%) p
Children with at least one of the following problems: 482 (50.3) 364 (38.0) < .001
Irritability 186 (19.3) 152 (15.8) < .001
Inability to stop playing video games 260 (27.1) 158 (16.5) < .001
Frequent fights with siblings 265 (27.6) 189 (19.7) < .001
Excessive dependence on parents 73 (7.6) 72 (7.5) .93
Refusal to sleep independently 14 (1.5) 18 (2.5) < .001

Note. Chi-squared tests were used to compare groups

Characteristics of children with behavioral problems during school closure

Children with behavioral problems during school closure (n = 482) Children without behavioral problems during school closure (n= 477) With problems vs. Without problems p
Gender Boys Girls Boys Girls
253/482 229/482 231/477 246/477 0.220 †
p = 1.00 *† p = 1.00 *†
1 83 94
2 85 79
Grade 3 85 79 0.675 †
4 79 65
5 66 76
6 84 84
Mean duration of watching
3.3 h (1.9) 2.8 h (1.8) < .001 ǂ
TV/video (SD)
Mean duration of playing 2.1 h (1.9) 1.4 h (1.6) < .001 ǂ
video games (SD)
Total screen time (SD) 5.4 h (2.7) 4.2 h (2.5) < .001 ǂ
Mean sleep duration (SD) 9.4 h (0.9) 9.4 h (0.8) 0.653 ǂ
Mean wakeup time (SD) 7:28 (1.0 h) 7:21 (1.0 h) 0.109 ǂ
Mean bedtime (SD) 21:54 (1.2 h) 21:54 (0.9 h) 0.357 ǂ
Bodyweight gain during school closure 105/482 56/477 < .001 †

Note. SD = standard deviation; h = hours; *: p compares proportion of boys and that of girls; †: Chi-squared tests were used to compare groups;

ǂ: Wilcoxon rank sum tests were used to compare groups

Relationship between screen time and behavioral problems

In general, schoolchildren watched TV and/or video and played video games significantly longer during school closure than after school reopened (TV/video: Mean [M] = 3.0 h, Standard Deviation [SD] =1.9 h versus M = 1.5 h, SD = 1.0 h; video games: M = 1.8 h, SD =1.8 h versus M = 0.8 h, SD = 0.8 h, respectively; p < .001). Total screen time during school closure was significantly higher in children with behavioral problems (+1.2 h; p < .001; Table 2) than in those without. During school closure, children with behavioral problems watched TV and/or videos, and played video games significantly longer than children without such problems (+0.5 h, +0.7 h, respectively; p < .001; Table 2). The logistic regression analysis revealed that both the time spent watching TV/video and playing video games were risk factors for behavioral problems during school closure (Table 3).

Odds ratios and 95% confidence intervals of exhibiting behavioral problems during school closure

Crude Adjusted
Variable OR 95% CI P value OR 95% CI P value
Time spent watching TV/video 1.1 1.07 - 1.22 < .001 1.2 1.07-1.24 < .001
Time spent playing video games 1.3 1.2 - 1.4 < .001 1.3 1.18 - 1.41 < .001
Total screen time 1.2 1.14 - 1.28 < .001 1.2 1.14 - 1.28 < .001
Bodyweight gain 2.1 1.47 - 2.98 < .001 2.0 1.43 - 2.92 < .001

Note. Logistic regression analyses were performed to calculate the adjusted odds ratio (OR) with 95% confidenceinterval (CI) for the relationship between exhibiting behavioral problems and screen time (time spent watching TVand/or videos, time spent playing video games and total screen time during school closure) and experiencingbodyweight gain during school closure, respectively. Controls are gender and grades

Association between sleep habits and behavioral problems

We examined children’s sleep habits during school closure and after schools reopened. In general, children slept longer during school closure than after schools reopened (M = 9.4 h, SD = 0.8 h versus M = 8.8 h, SD = 0.7 h; p < .001). Mean sleep duration during school closure did not differ between children with and without behavioral problems (Table 2). Moreover, bedtime and wakeup time during school closure were almost identical between these children (Table 2).

Bodyweight change

Overall, 16.7% of the children reported bodyweight gain (n = 161; 79 boys, 82 girls), and 0.3% reported bodyweight loss (n = 3, all boys) during school closure (Table 4). We assessed the relationship between bodyweight gain and screen time during school closure. Children with bodyweight gain reported higher total screen time, which was attributed to longer duration of watching TV/video, but not that of playing video games (Table 4). Thus, time spent watching TV/video was a risk factor for bodyweight gain and not time spent playing video games (Table 5). Sleep habits did not differ between these children (Table 4).

Characteristics of children with experiencing bodyweight gain

Children with bodyweight gain (n = 161) Children no bodyweight gain (n= 798) With bodyweight gain vs. no bodyweight gain p
Boys Girls Boys Girls
Gender 79/161 82/161 405/798 393/798 0.730 †
p = 1.00 *† p = 1.00 *†
1 83 94
2 85 79
Grade 3 85 79 0.675†
4 79 65
5 66 76
6 84 84
Mean duration of watching TV/video (SD) 3.6 h (2.3) 2.9 h (1.8) < .001 ǂ
Mean duration of playing video games (SD) 1.9 h (2.0) 1.7 h (1.7) < .674 ǂ
Total screen time (SD) 5.2 h (3.0) 4.7 h (2.5) < .001 ǂ
Mean sleep duration (SD) 9.5 h (0.8) 9.4 h (0.8) 0.234 ǂ
Mean wakeup time (SD) 7:28 (1.0 h) 7:21 (1.0 h) 0.402 ǂ
Mean bedtime (SD) 21:54 (0.9 h) 21:54 (0.9 h) 0.490 ǂ
Bodyweight gain during school closure 105/161 377/798 < .001 †

Note. SD = standard deviation; h = hours; *: p compares proportion of boys and that of girls; †: Chi-squared tests were used to compare groups;

ǂ: Wilcoxon rank sum tests were used to compare groups

Odds ratio and 95% confidence intervals of children experiencing body weight gain during school closure

Crude Adjusted
Variable OR 95% CI P value OR 95% CI P value
Time spent watching TV/video 1.2 1.10 - 1.30 < .001 1.2 1.08 - 1.28 < .001
Time spent playing video games 1.1 0.96 - 1.15 0.317 1.0 0.91 - 1.12 0.845
Total screen time 1.1 1.05 - 1.19 < .001 1.1 1.02 - 1.16 < .001
Behavioral problems 2.1 1.47 - 2.98 < .001 2.0 1.43 - 2.92 < .001

Note. Logistic regression analyses were performed to calculate the adjusted odds ratio (OR) with 95% confidence interval (CI) for the relationship between experiencing body weight gain during school closure and screen time (time spent watching TV and/or videos, time spent playing video games and total screen time during school closure) and exhibiting behavioral problems during school closure, respectively. Controls are gender and grades

The interrelationship between behavioral problems and bodyweight gain

Lastly, we assessed the relationship between bodyweight gain and behavioral problems during school closure. The proportion of children experiencing bodyweight gain during school closure was significantly higher among those with behavioral problems than those without (Table 2). Similarly, children with bodyweight gain reported a significantly higher proportion of behavioral problems than those with no bodyweight gain (Table 4). Logistic regression analyses revealed that exhibiting behavioral problems during school closure and experiencing bodyweight gain were mutual risk factors (Tables 3 and 5).

Discussion

In the current study, we revealed that more than half of schoolchildren in Miyaki-Machi, Japan, exhibited some behavioral problems during COVID-19 school closure, which decreased after schools reopened. School closure due to COVID-19 poses an imminent threat to children’s health and wellbeing worldwide, in educational (8), nutritional (9), and economic terms (9). This study also reinforces the results of prior reports (2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9) demonstrating negative impacts of school closure on children, especially children’s mental health.

School closure due to COVID-19 were unique in some respects. In Japan, only 5% of all public elementary schools provided interactive online classes in April, 2020 (10). The remaining schoolchildren were given paper-based homework (10) without time constraints. These children might have lacked direct connections with teachers and schools for approximately three months, which was longer than usual annual school closures. The four elementary schools examined in this study had no remote learning systems. Additionally, it was reported that only 20.8% of elementary schoolchildren in Japan used daycare in 2020 (11). The remaining children were ordered to stay at home and were prohibited to contact their friends, indicating that they could not engage in enjoyable activities with their friends. Although teachers in these elementary schools sometimes visited their students, compared with the usual annual closures, most children were likely placed in an environment in which they could spend time on their own terms, without an appropriate educational connection to school. Thus, children likely both feared infection and experienced distress due to experiencing unstructured days, frustration, boredom (12, 13, 14, 15), and isolation (15) for longer periods than during regular annual school closures. This was one of the possible factors causing schoolchildren in this area to experience negative impacts due to school closure.

A national survey in the US revealed that 14% parents reported worsening behavior in their children (4) and increases in their children’s irritability, inattention, and clinging since the pandemic began (7), regardless of age (16). A survey in China showed that 22.6% of students (from Grade 2 to Grade 6) reported depressive symptoms, and 18.9% reported anxiety during in-home confinement (17). Our study showed that more than 50% of schoolchildren, regardless of their gender and grades, exhibited behavioral problems during school closure, and the percentage decreased significantly after schools reopened. This indicates that these children exhibited behavioral problems in response to the stress experienced during school closure. Reopening schools might have relieved children of these stressors through school-based peer interactions and reestablishment of their daily routines (18). A significant increase in the body mass index of youths during COVID-19 lockdown has been reported (3). Notably, 16.7% of children experienced bodyweight gain during school closure in this study. Delayed access to healthcare (19), delayed illness presentation (19, 20), and dropping vaccination rates (21) have been reported as the indirect physical effects of the COVID-19 pandemic in children. In this study, we demonstrated that children exhibiting behavioral problems during school closure had a high risk of bodyweight gain. Mental burden originating from this COVID-19 school closure might have affected children’s physical status. Thus, school closure exerted profound negative impacts on children, both psychologically and physically.

Although media entertainment has become a tool for coping with distress, boredom, and lack of social interaction caused by the pandemic (16, 22), digital technology has demonstrated potentially harmful effects on the developing brain (23). Moreover, total screen time is associated with unfavorable temperament (24), such as irritability, anxiety, clinginess, and inattention (23, 25). Therefore, we examined screen time during this COVID-19 school closure. Children’s screen time in this study was longer than that reported in previous studies (3). In addition, children with behavioral problems during school closure had a longer total screen time than those without such problems. This suggests that children with behavioral problems experienced the additional distress of excessive usage of screen media in addition to the distress of the quarantine.

Media use, especially TV viewing (26), is associated with the risk of becoming overweight (27). The current study results are consistent with those of previous studies (26, 27). The bodyweight gain during school closure reported in this study may have resulted from longer screen time and less physical activity due to the quarantine (27, 28). The type of screen time has been shown to affect physical and psychological health; moreover, the magnitude of the effect depends on how children use screens— passively (e.g., watching TV) or interactively (e.g., playing video games) (24). The former exerts negative impacts on schoolchildren (24). Consistent with these findings, the current study demonstrated that passive use, and not interactive use, of screen media affected schoolchildren physically. However, both the passive and interactive use of screen media were significantly associated with children exhibiting behavioral problems during school closure. Despite the lack of investigation of the video game genre played by children during school closure in the current study, some violent games profoundly affected children’s psychological status (29, 30). Although gaming alone and gaming in groups were not associated with depressive symptoms in adolescents (31), it might be better for children to watch TV with other people rather than watch it alone (32). As children were prohibited from contacting non-family members amidst the COVID-19 school closure, it might be possible that screen time exerted more profound effects then in comparison to during the annual school closure (33). Research suggests that educational efficacy in remote learning systems is higher with interactive communication than one way communication (34). This may indicate the need to nurture human connection—families and friends—even in screen media usage for mental health (35), especially among children. Therefore, our study highlights the importance of precisely how children interact with screen media during long school closures, such as summer vacations and COVID-19 quarantine.

We further investigated other physical and psychological factors affecting children’s sleep habits. Sleep plays a critical role in children’s physical and mental development; shorter sleep duration is a risk factor for behavioral issues (36). The American Academy of Sleep Medicine recommends that children between 6 and 12 years of age should sleep for more than 9 hours per night (37). Japan has been reported to be the most sleepless country in the world (38); sleep duration among most schoolchildren in Japan is less than 9 hours per night (36). Children in our study appeared to sleep sufficiently to satisfy their physiological needs during school closure. Before implementing this study, we speculated that children with behavioral problems would stay up late at night and engage in longer screen time than those without such problems. However, sleep duration in children with behavioral problems did not differ from that in those without behavioral problems. Bedtime and wakeup time were almost identical in both types of children. Furthermore, sleep habits were not associated with bodyweight gain during school closure. These observations indicate that shorter sleep duration was not the only factor associated with the physical and mental health of children in this study. Although this study did not examine to what degree parents could supervise their children’s screen time and sleep habits during school closure, it may be necessary for children to be assigned a daily routine by their parents or guardians (26, 27, 39, 40).

The strength of our study is that it was conducted with participants from public elementary schools. Compared to online surveys, our study was not affected by Internet access or access to devices. In contrast, the primary limitation of this study was that the questionnaire was originally developed by the authors; thus, the study lacked standardized and validated instruments. This caused potential differences in interpretation of variable meaning across respondents and made it difficult to generalize our findings globally. The second limitation was a lack of information about each child’s environment, such as family member or parents’ job, which may influence the results (5). The third limitation was the potential inadequacy of the methods and items used to assess children exhibiting behavioral problems. Moreover, the questionnaires did not include items on psychosomatic symptoms or psychological symptoms such as depression and inattention. Lastly, to investigate and compare the long-term mental impact of school closures on children due to this global pandemic, especially as it relates to screen usage, we must use well-generalized instruments designed to investigate the mental health status of children and evaluate regional differences in quarantine-sternness while considering the influences of gender and grades.

Clinical significance

The school closure due to COVID-19 had negative physical and psychological impacts on schoolchildren in Japan, which were closely associated with each other and with children’s screen media usage. Accordingly, parents should restrict children’s arbitrary use of screen media; in addition, they should provide children with the opportunity to use screen media interactively and spend enjoyable screen time together with their children to establish human connection. For future school closures, we suggest that parents and caregivers cooperate with teachers to generate daily routines for children, which enable the latter to feel connected to friends and schools, rather than allow children to engage in arbitrary screen time.

eISSN:
2245-8875
Lingua:
Inglese
Frequenza di pubblicazione:
Volume Open
Argomenti della rivista:
Medicine, Basic Medical Science, other