Accesso libero

Unification Strategies in Cognitive Science

  
16 mar 2017
INFORMAZIONI SU QUESTO ARTICOLO

Cita
Scarica la copertina

Anderson, J. R. (2007). How Can the Mind Occur in the Physical Universe? Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195324259.001.0001 Search in Google Scholar

Arbib, M. A. (2005). From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(2), 105-24-67. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0500003810.1017/S0140525X05000038 Search in Google Scholar

Arbib, M. A. (2012). How the brain got language: the mirror system hypothesis. New York: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:osobl/9780199896684.001.0001 Search in Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417-423. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-210.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2 Search in Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working Memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 47-89). New York / San Francisco / London: Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-110.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1 Search in Google Scholar

Bechtel, W. (1986). The Nature of Scientific Integration. In W. Bechtel (Ed.), Integrating Scientific Disciplines (pp. 3-52). Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-010-9435-1 110.1007/978-94-010-9435-1 Search in Google Scholar

Bechtel, W. (2008). Mental Mechanisms. New York: Routledge (Taylor & Francis Group). Search in Google Scholar

Bechtel, W. (2016). Investigating neural representations: the tale of place cells. Synthese, 193(5), 1287-1321. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-014-0480-810.1007/s11229-014-0480-8 Search in Google Scholar

Bechtel, W., & McCauley, R. N. (1999). Heuristic identity theory (or back to the future): The mind-body problem against the background of research strategies in cognitive neuroscience. In Proceedings of the 21st Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 67-72). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Search in Google Scholar

Benhamou, S. (1996). No evidence for cognitive mapping in rats. Animal Behaviour, 52(1), 201-212.10.1006/anbe.1996.0165 Search in Google Scholar

Bickle, J. (1998). Psychoneural reduction the new wave. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. Search in Google Scholar

Boone,W., & Piccinini, G. (2015). The cognitive neuroscience revolution. Synthese. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-015-0783-410.1007/s11229-015-0783-4 Search in Google Scholar

Busemeyer, J. R., & Diederich, A. (2010). Cognitive modeling. Los Angeles: Sage. Search in Google Scholar

Byrne, M. D. (2012). Unified theories of cognition.Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 3(4), 431-438. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.118010.1002/wcs.1180 Search in Google Scholar

Chaitin, G. J. (1987). Algorithmic information theory. Cambridge [Cambridgeshire]; New York: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511608858 Search in Google Scholar

Churchland, P. M. (1985). Reduction, qualia, and the direct introspection of brain states. The Journal of Philosophy, 82(1), 8-28.10.2307/2026509 Search in Google Scholar

Clark, A. (2013).Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(3), 181-204. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X1200047710.1017/S0140525X12000477 Search in Google Scholar

Clark, A. (2016). Surfing uncertainty: prediction, action, and the embodied mind.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190217013.001.0001 Search in Google Scholar

Cooper, R. P. (2007). The Role of Falsification in the Development of Cognitive Architectures: Insights from a Lakatosian Analysis. Cognitive Science, 31(3), 509-533. https://doi.org/10.1080/1532690070132659210.1080/15326900701326592 Search in Google Scholar

Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1987). From Evolution to Behavior: Evolutionary Psychology as the Missing Link. In J. Dupre (Ed.), The Latest on the Best. Essays on Evolution and Optimality (pp. 277-303). Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. Search in Google Scholar

Craver, C. F. (2002). Interlevel experiments and multilevel mechanisms in the neuroscience of memory. Philosophy of Science, 69(S3), 83-97.10.1086/341836 Search in Google Scholar

Craver, C. F. (2007a). Explaining the Brain. Mechanisms and the mosaic unity of neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199299317.003.0007 Search in Google Scholar

Craver, C. F. (2007b). Explaining the Brain. Mechanisms and the mosaic unity of neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199299317.003.0007 Search in Google Scholar

Craver, C. F., & Darden, L. (2013). In search of mechanisms: discoveries across the life sciences.10.7208/chicago/9780226039824.001.0001 Search in Google Scholar

Cummins, R. (1984). Functional analysis. Conceptual Issues in Evolutionary Biology: An Anthology. Search in Google Scholar

Cummins, R. (2000). “How does it work” versus “what are the laws?”: Two conceptions of psychological explanation. In F. Keil & R. A. Wilson (Eds.), Explanation and Cognition (pp. 117-145). Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. Search in Google Scholar

Danks, D. (2014). Unifying the mind: cognitive representations as graphical models. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/9540.001.0001 Search in Google Scholar

Darden, L., & Maull, N. (1977). Interfield Theories. Philosophy of Science, 44(1), 43-64.10.1086/288723 Search in Google Scholar

Dawson, M. (1998). Understanding cognitive science. Malden Mass.: Blackwell. Search in Google Scholar

Derdikman, D., & Moser, E. I. (2010). A manifold of spatial maps in the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(12), 561-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.00410.1016/j.tics.2010.09.004 Search in Google Scholar

di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research, 91(1), 176-80.10.1007/BF00230027 Search in Google Scholar

Eliasmith, C. (2013). How to build the brain: a neural architecture for biological cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199794546.001.0001 Search in Google Scholar

Eliasmith, C., Stewart, T. C., Choo, X., Bekolay, T., DeWolf, T., Tang, Y., ... Rasmussen, D. (2012). A Large-Scale Model of the Functioning Brain. Science, 338(6111), 1202-1205. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.122526610.1126/science.1225266 Search in Google Scholar

Fodor, J. A. (1968). Psychological explanation: an introduction to the philosophy of psychology. New York: Random House. Search in Google Scholar

Forster, M., & Sober, E. (1994). How to Tell When Simpler, More Unified, or Less Ad Hoc Theories will Provide More Accurate Predictions. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 45(1), 1-35. https://doi.org/10.1093/bjps/45.1.110.1093/bjps/45.1.1 Search in Google Scholar

Gallese, V. (2003). The Roots of Empathy: The Shared Manifold Hypothesis and the Neural Basis of Intersubjectivity. Psychopathology, 36(4), 171-180. https://doi.org/10.1159/00007278610.1159/000072786 Search in Google Scholar

Glennan, S. S. (2002). Rethinking Mechanistic Explanation. Philosophy of Science, 69(S3), S342-S353. https://doi.org/10.1086/34185710.1086/341857 Search in Google Scholar

Goodman, N. (1961). Safety, Strength, Simplicity. Philosophy of Science, 28(2), 150-151. https://doi.org/10.1086/28779510.1086/287795 Search in Google Scholar

Hempel, C., & Oppenheim, P. (1948). Studies in the Logic of Explanation. Philosophy of Science, 15(2), 135-175.10.1086/286983 Search in Google Scholar

Hensel, W. M. (2013). On Reduction and Interfield Integration in Neuroscience. In M. Miłkowski & K. Talmont-Kamiński (Eds.), Regarding the Mind, Naturally: Naturalist Approaches to the Sciences of the Mental (pp. 167-181). Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing. Search in Google Scholar

Hickok, G. (2014). The myth of mirror neurons: the real neuroscience of communication and cognition. New York: WW Norton. Search in Google Scholar

Hochstein, E. (2015). One mechanism, many models: a distributed theory of mechanistic explanation. Synthese. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-015-0844-810.1007/s11229-015-0844-8 Search in Google Scholar

Hooker, C. A. (1981a). Towards a General Theory of Reduction. Part I: Historical and Scientific Setting. Dialogue, 20(1), 38-59. https://doi.org/10.1017/S001221730002308810.1017/S0012217300023088 Search in Google Scholar

Hooker, C. A. (1981b). Towards a General Theory of Reduction. Part II: Identity in Reduction. Dialogue, 20(2), 201-236. https://doi.org/10.1017/S001221730002330110.1017/S0012217300023301 Search in Google Scholar

Hooker, C. A. (1981c). Towards a General Theory of Reduction. Part III: Cross- Categorical Reduction. Dialogue, 20(3), 496-529. https://doi.org/10.1017/S001221730002359310.1017/S0012217300023593 Search in Google Scholar

Hora, J., & Campos, P. (2015). A review of performance criteria to validate simulation models. Expert Systems, 32(5), 578-595. https://doi.org/10.1111/exsy.1211110.1111/exsy.12111 Search in Google Scholar

Kitcher, P. (1989). Explanatory Unification and the Causal Structure of the World. In P. Kitcher & W. C. Salmon (Eds.), Scientific Explanation (Vol. 505, pp. 410-505). Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press. Search in Google Scholar

Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umiltà, M. A., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Hearing sounds, understanding actions: action representation in mirror neurons. Science (New York, N.Y.), 297(5582), 846-8. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.107031110.1126/science.1070311 Search in Google Scholar

Laird, J. E., Newell, A., & Rosenbloom, P. S. (1987). SOAR: An architecture for general intelligence. Artificial Intelligence, 33(1), 1-64. https://doi.org/10.1016/0004-3702(87)90050-610.1016/0004-3702(87)90050-6 Search in Google Scholar

Li, M., & Vitanyi, P. (1993). An Introduction to Kolmogorov Complexity and Its Applications. New York, Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag.10.1007/978-1-4757-3860-5 Search in Google Scholar

McCulloch, W. S., & Pitts, W. (1943). A Logical Calculus of the Ideas Immanent in Nervous Activity. Bulletin of Mathematical Biophysics, 5, 115-133.10.1007/BF02478259 Search in Google Scholar

Metta, G., Natale, L., Nori, F., Sandini, G., Vernon, D., Fadiga, L., ... Montesano, L. (2010). The iCub humanoid robot: An open-systems platform for research in cognitive development. Neural Networks, 23(8), 1125-1134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neunet.2010.08.01010.1016/j.neunet.2010.08.010 Search in Google Scholar

Miłkowski,M. (2013). Explaining the Computational Mind. Cambridge,Mass.:MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/9339.001.0001 Search in Google Scholar

Morse, A. F., Herrera, C., Clowes, R., Montebelli, A., & Ziemke, T. (2011). The role of robotic modelling in cognitive science. New Ideas in Psychology, 29(3), 312-324. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2011.02.00110.1016/j.newideapsych.2011.02.001 Search in Google Scholar

Mouras, H., Stol´eru, S., Moulier, V., P´el´egrini-Issac, M., Rouxel, R., Grandjean, B., ... Bittoun, J. (2008). Activation of mirror-neuron system by erotic video clips predicts degree of induced erection: an fMRI study. NeuroImage, 42, 1142-1150. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.05110.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.051 Search in Google Scholar

Newell, A. (1973). You can’t play 20 questions with nature and win: Projective comments on the papers of this symposium. In W. G. Chase (Ed.), Visual information processing (pp. 283-308). New York: Academic Press. Search in Google Scholar

Newell, A. (1990). Unified Theories of Cognition. Cambridge, Mass. and London: Harvard University Press. Search in Google Scholar

O’Reilly, R. C., & Munakata, Y. (2000). Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience. Understanding the Mind by Simulating the Brain. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/2014.001.0001 Search in Google Scholar

Piccinini, G., & Craver, C. F. (2011). Integrating psychology and neuroscience: functional analyses as mechanism sketches. Synthese, 183(3), 283-311. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-011-9898-410.1007/s11229-011-9898-4 Search in Google Scholar

Ponseti, J., Bosinski, H. A., Wolff, S., Peller, M., Jansen, O., Mehdorn, H. M., ... Siebner, H. R. (2006). A functional endophenotype for sexual orientation in humans. NeuroImage, 33(3), 825-833. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.00210.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.002 Search in Google Scholar

Popper, K. R. (1959). The logic of scientific discovery. New Yorker, The. Hutchinson.10.1063/1.3060577 Search in Google Scholar

Quine, W. V. (1948). On What There Is. The Review of Metaphysics, 2(5), 21-38. Search in Google Scholar

Ramón y Cajal, S. (1990). New ideas on the structure of the nervous system in man and vertebrates. (L. W. Swanson, Trans.). Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. Search in Google Scholar

Richardson, R. C. (2007). Evolutionary Psychology as Maladapted Psychology. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/7464.001.0001 Search in Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169-92. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.14423010.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230 Search in Google Scholar

Roberts, S., & Pashler, H. (2000). How persuasive is a good fit? A comment on theory testing. Psychological Review, 107(2), 358.10.1037/0033-295X.107.2.358 Search in Google Scholar

Salmon, W. C. (1998). Causality and explanation. Oxford University Press, USA.10.1093/0195108647.001.0001 Search in Google Scholar

Schaffner, K. F. (1993). Discovery and explanation in biology and medicine. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Search in Google Scholar

Schurz, G. (1991). Relevant Deduction: From Solving Paradoxes Towards a General Theory. Erkenntnis, 35, 391-437. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF0038829510.1007/BF00388295 Search in Google Scholar

Thagard, P. (2000). Coherence in thought and action. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/1900.001.0001 Search in Google Scholar

Thagard, P. (2007). Coherence, Truth, and the Development of Scientific Knowledge. Philosophy of Science, 74, 28-47.10.1086/520941 Search in Google Scholar

Van Orden, G. C., & Kloos, H. (2003). The Module Mistake. Cortex, 39(1), 164-166. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70092-310.1016/S0010-9452(08)70092-3 Search in Google Scholar

Votsis, I. (2015). Unification: Not Just a Thing of Beauty. THEORIA. An International Journal for Theory, History and Foundations of Science, 30(1), 97. https://doi.org/10.1387/theoria.1269510.1387/theoria.12695 Search in Google Scholar

Weisberg, M. (2006). Robustness Analysis. Philosophy of Science, 73(5), 730-742. https://doi.org/10.1086/51862810.1086/518628 Search in Google Scholar

Zenil, H. (2010). Compression-based investigation of the dynamical properties of cellular automata and other systems. Journal of Complex Systems, 19(1).10.25088/ComplexSystems.19.1.1 Search in Google Scholar

Lingua:
Inglese
Frequenza di pubblicazione:
4 volte all'anno
Argomenti della rivista:
Filosofia, Filosofia, altro