Accesso libero

Neurobiology of Consciousness: Current Research and Perspectives

   | 12 feb 2016
INFORMAZIONI SU QUESTO ARTICOLO

Cita

1. Abraham, A., “The world according to me: personal relevance and the medial prefrontal cortex”, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 2013, pp. 341.10.3389/fnhum.2013.00341369845523847510Search in Google Scholar

2. Allen, A.K., Wilkins, K., Gazzaley, A., Morsella, E., “Conscious thoughts from reflex-like processes: a new experimental paradigm for consciousness research”, Consciousness and Cognition 22(4), 2013, pp. 1318-1331.10.1016/j.concog.2013.08.00724056177Search in Google Scholar

3. Amico, E., Gomez, F., Di Perri, C., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Lesenfants, D., Boveroux, P., Bonhomme, V., Brichant, J.F., Marinazzo, D., Laureys, S., “Posterior cingulate cortex-related co-activation patterns: a resting state FMRI study in propofol-induced loss of consciousness”, PLoS One. 9(6), 2014, pp. e100012.10.1371/journal.pone.0100012407618424979748Search in Google Scholar

4. Andrews-Hanna, J.R., Smallwood, J., Spreng, R.N., “The default network and selfgenerated thought: component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance”, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1316, 2014, pp. 29-52.10.1111/nyas.12360403962324502540Search in Google Scholar

5. Arroyo-Anlló, E.M., Bouston, A.T., Fargeau, M.N., Orgaz Baz, B., Gil, R., “Self- Consciousness in Patients with Behavioral Variant Frontotemporal Dementia”, Journal of Alzheimer's Disease 2015 Nov 23. [Epub ahead of print]10.3233/JAD-15082126599058Search in Google Scholar

6. Aru, J., Bachmann, T., “Phenomenal awareness can emerge without attention”, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 2013, pp. 891.10.3389/fnhum.2013.00891386890824391577Search in Google Scholar

7. Aru, J., Bachmann, T., “Still wanted - the mechanisms of consciousness!”, Frontiers in Psychology 6, 2015, pp. 5.10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00005430086425653636Search in Google Scholar

8. Aspell, J.E., Lenggenhager, B., Blanke, O., “Keeping in touch with one's self: multisensory mechanisms of self-consciousness”, PLoS One 4(8), 2009, pp. e6488.10.1371/journal.pone.0006488271516519654862Search in Google Scholar

9. Baars, B.J., A cognitive theory of consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1988.Search in Google Scholar

10. Baars, B.J., In the theater of consciousness. New York: Oxford University Press, 1997.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195102659.001.1Search in Google Scholar

11. Bachmann, T., “Attention as a process of selection, perception as a process of representation, and phenomenal experience as the resulting process of perception being modulated by a dedicated consciousness mechanism”, Frontiers in Psychology 2, 2011, pp. 387.10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00387324768022232612Search in Google Scholar

12. Bachmann, T., “How to begin to overcome the ambiguity present in differentiation between contents and levels of consciousness?”, Frontiers in Psychology 3, 2012, pp. 82.10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00082Search in Google Scholar

13. Bachmann, T., Hudetz, A.G., “It is time to combine the two main traditions in the research on the neural correlates of consciousness: C = L × D”, Frontiers in Psychology 5, 2014, pp. 940.10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00940Search in Google Scholar

14. Baddeley, A.D., Allen, R.J., Hitch, G.J., “Binding in visual working memory: the role of the episodic buffer”, Neuropsychologia 49, 2011, pp. 1393-1400.10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.042Search in Google Scholar

15. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., Frith, U., “Does the autistic child have a "theory of mind"?”, Cognition 21(1), 1985, pp. 37-46.10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8Search in Google Scholar

16. Bayne, T., “Self-consciousness and the Unity of Consciousness”, Monist 87 (2), 2004, pp. 219-236.10.5840/monist200487210Search in Google Scholar

17. Berkovich-Ohana, A., Glicksohn, J., “The consciousness state space (CSS)-a unifying model for consciousness and self”, Frontiers in Psychology 5, 2014, pp. 341.10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00341401078924808870Search in Google Scholar

18. Blanke, O., “Multisensory brain mechanisms of bodily self-consciousness”, Nature Reviews Neuroscience 13(8), 2012, pp. 556-571.10.1038/nrn329222805909Search in Google Scholar

19. Blanke, O., Metzinger, T., “Full-body illusions and minimal phenomenal selfhood”, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13(1), 2009, pp. 7-13.10.1016/j.tics.2008.10.00319058991Search in Google Scholar

20. Blanke, O., Slater, M., Serino, A., “Behavioral, neural, and computational principles of bodily self-consciousness”, Neuron 88(1), 2015, pp. 145-166.10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.02926447578Search in Google Scholar

21. Block, N., “On a confusion about a function of consciousness”, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18, 1995, pp. 227-247.10.1017/S0140525X00038188Search in Google Scholar

22. Block, N., “How can we find the neural correlate of consciousness?”, Trends in Neuroscience 19(11), 1996, pp. 456-9.Search in Google Scholar

23. Block, N., “Two neural correlates of consciousness”, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9(2), 2005, pp. 46-52.10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.00615668096Search in Google Scholar

24. Bonfiglio, L., Piarulli, A., Olcese, U., Andre, P., Arrighi, P., Frisoli, A., Rossi, B., Bergamasco, M., Carboncini, M.C., “Spectral parameters modulation and source localization of blink-related alpha and low-beta oscillations differentiate minimally conscious state from vegetative state/unresponsive wakefulness syndrome”, PLoS One. 9(3), 2014, pp. e93252. Erratum in: PLoS One. 9(4), pp. e95948.10.1371/journal.pone.0093252397099024676098Search in Google Scholar

25. Brewer, J.A., Garrison, K.A., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., “What about the "self" is processed in the posterior cingulate cortex?”, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 2013, pp. 647. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00647378834724106472Search in Google Scholar

26. Christoff, K., Cosmelli, D., Legrand, D., Thompson, E., “Specifying the self for cognitive neuroscience”, Trends in Cognitive Science 15(3), 2011, pp. 104-112.10.1016/j.tics.2011.01.00121288760Search in Google Scholar

27. Cohen, M.A., Konkle, T., Rhee, J.Y., Nakayama, K., Alvarez G.A., “Processing multiple visual objects is limited by overlap in neural channels”, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 111(24), 2014, pp. 8955-60.10.1073/pnas.1317860111406650624889618Search in Google Scholar

28. Cowan, N., “Multiple concurrent thoughts: The meaning and developmental neuropsychology of working memory”, Developmental Neuropsychology 35(5), 2010, pp. 447-474.10.1080/87565641.2010.494985292529520721769Search in Google Scholar

29. Crick F., Koch C., “Towards a neurobiological theory of consciousness”, Seminars in the Neurosciences 2, 1990, pp. 263-275.Search in Google Scholar

30. Crick, F., Koch, C., “Consciousness and neuroscience”, Cerebral Cortex 8(2), 1998, pp. 97-107.10.1093/cercor/8.2.979542889Search in Google Scholar

31. D'Argembeau, A., Ruby, P., Collette, F., Degueldre, C., Balteau, E., Luxen, A., Maquet, P., Salmon, E., “Distinct regions of the medial prefrontal cortex are associated with selfreferential processing and perspective taking”, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19(6), 2007, pp. 935-44.10.1162/jocn.2007.19.6.93517536964Search in Google Scholar

32. D’Argembeau, A., Feyers, D., Majerus, S., Collette, F., Van der Linden, M., Maquet, P., Salmon, E., “Self-reflection across time: cortical midline structures differentiate between present and past selves”, Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 3(3), 2008, pp. 244-25.10.1093/scan/nsn020256676919015116Search in Google Scholar

33. D'Argembeau, A., Cassol, H., Phillips, C., Balteau, E., Salmon, E., Van der Linden, M., “Brains creating stories of selves: the neural basis of autobiographical reasoning”, Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 9(5), 2014, pp. 646-52.10.1093/scan/nst028401410123482628Search in Google Scholar

34. DaSilveira, A., DeSouza, M.L., Gomes, W.B., “Self-consciousness concept and assessment in self-report measures”, Frontiers in Psychology 6, 2015, pp. 930.10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00930449021326191030Search in Google Scholar

35. Davis, J.I., Markman, A.B., “Embodied cognition as a practical paradigm: introduction to the topic, the future of embodied cognition”, Topics in Cognitive Science 4(4), 2012, pp. 685-91.10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01227.x23060128Search in Google Scholar

36. Dehaene, S., Sergent, C., Changeux, J.-P., “A neuronal network model linking subjective reports and objective physiological data during conscious perception”, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 100(14), 2003, pp. 8520-8525.10.1073/pnas.133257410016626112829797Search in Google Scholar

37. Demertzi, A., Gómez, F., Crone, J.S., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Tshibanda, L., Noirhomme, Q., Thonnard, M., Charland-Verville, V., Kirsch, M., Laureys, S., Soddu, A., “Multiple fMRI system-level baseline connectivity is disrupted in patients with consciousness alterations”, Cortex 52, 2014, pp. 35-46.10.1016/j.cortex.2013.11.00524480455Search in Google Scholar

38. Demertzi, A., Antonopoulos, G., Heine, L., Voss, H.U., Crone, J.S., de Los Angeles, C., Bahri, M.A., Di Perri, C., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Charland-Verville, V., Kronbichler, M., Trinka, E., Phillips, C., Gomez, F., Tshibanda, L., Soddu, A., Schiff, N.D., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Laureys, S., “Intrinsic functional connectivity differentiates minimally conscious from unresponsive patients”, Brain 138(Pt 9), 2015, pp. 2619-31.10.1093/brain/awv16926117367Search in Google Scholar

39. De Sousa, A., “Towards an integrative theory of consciousness: part 1 (neurobiological and cognitive models)”, Mens Sana Monographs 11(1), 2013, pp. 100-150.10.4103/0973-1229.109335365321923678241Search in Google Scholar

40. De Sousa, A., “Towards an integrative theory of consciousness: part 2 (an anthology of various other models)”, Mens Sana Monographs 11(1), 2013, pp. 151-209.10.4103/0973-1229.109341365322023678242Search in Google Scholar

41. Di Perri, C., Thibaut, A., Heine, L., Soddu, A., Demertzi, A., Laureys, S., “Measuring consciousness in coma and related states”, World Journal of Radiology 6(8), 2014, pp. 589-97.10.4329/wjr.v6.i8.589414743925170396Search in Google Scholar

42. Edelman, G.M., Gally, J.A., Baars, J., “Biology of consciousness”, Frontiers in Psychology 2, 2011, pp. 4.10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00004311144421713129Search in Google Scholar

43. Fabbro, F., Aglioti, S.M., Bergamasco, M., Clarici, A., Panksepp, J., “Evolutionary aspects of self- and world consciousness in vertebrates”, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9, 2015, pp. 157. 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00157437462525859205Search in Google Scholar

44. Faivre N, Salomon R, Blanke O., “Visual consciousness and bodily selfconsciousness”, Current Opinions in Neurology 28(1), 2015, pp. 23-28.10.1097/WCO.0000000000000160Search in Google Scholar

45. Feinberg, T.E., “Neuroontology, neurobiological naturalism, and consciousness: A challenge to scientific reduction and a solution”, Physics of Life Reviews 9, 2012, pp. 13-34.10.1016/j.plrev.2011.10.019Search in Google Scholar

46. Flagan, T., Beer, J.S., “Three ways in which midline regions contribute to selfevaluation”, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 2013, pp. 45.10.3389/fnhum.2013.00450Search in Google Scholar

47. Foster, P.P., “Role of physical and mental training in brain network configuration”, Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience 7, 2015, pp. 117.10.3389/fnagi.2015.00117Search in Google Scholar

48. Gallagher, S., “Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science”, Trends in Cognitive Science 4(1), 2000, pp. 14-21.10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01417-5Search in Google Scholar

49. Gallagher, S., “A pattern theory of self”, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 2013, pp. 443.10.3389/fnhum.2013.00443373011823914173Search in Google Scholar

50. Gallese, V., Lakoff, G., “The brain’s concepts: the role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge”, Cognitive Neuroscience 22(3/4), 2005, pp. 455-479.10.1080/0264329044200031021038261Search in Google Scholar

51. Gardiner, J.M., “Episodic memory and autonoetic consciousness: a first-person approach”, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 356(1413), 2001, pp. 1351-1361.Search in Google Scholar

52. Gierer, A., “Brain, mind and limitations of a scientific theory of human consciousness”, BioEssays 30, 2008, pp. 499-505.10.1002/bies.2074318404733Search in Google Scholar

53. Gil, R., Arroyo-Anllo, E.M., Ingrand, P., Gil, M., Neau, J.P., Ornon, C., Bonnaud, V., “Self-consciousness and Alzheimer's disease”, Acta Neurologica Scandinavica 104(5), 2001, pp. 296-300.10.1034/j.1600-0404.2001.00280.x11696024Search in Google Scholar

54. Gosseries, O., Schnakers, C., Ledoux, D., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Bruno, M.A., Demertzi, A., Noirhomme, Q., Lehembre, R., Damas, P., Goldman, S., Peeters, E., Moonen, G., Laureys, S., “Automated EEG entropy measurements in coma, vegetative state/unresponsive wakefulness syndrome and minimally conscious state”, Functional Neurology 26(1), 2011, 25-30.Search in Google Scholar

55. Guldenmund, P., Stender, J., Heine, L., Laureys, S., “Mindsight: diagnostics in disorders of consciousness”, Critical Care Research and Practice 2012, 2012, pp. 624724.10.1155/2012/624724350564023213492Search in Google Scholar

56. Guldenmund, P., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Boly, M., Laureys, S., Soddu, A.., “A default mode of brain function in altered states of consciousness”, Archives Italiennes de Biologie 150, 2012, pp. 107-21.Search in Google Scholar

57. Jacobs, C. Silvanto, J., “How is working memory content consciously experienced? The 'conscious copy' model of WM introspection”, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 55, 2015, pp. 510-519.10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.06.003Search in Google Scholar

58. Kanwisher, N., “Functional specificity in the human brain: a window into the functional architecture of the mind”, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107(25), 2010, pp. 11163-1170.10.1073/pnas.1005062107Search in Google Scholar

59. Keysers, C., Gazzola, V., “Expanding the mirror: vicarious activity for actions, emotions, and sensations”, Current Opinions in Neurobiology 19(6), 2009, pp. 666-671.10.1016/j.conb.2009.10.006Search in Google Scholar

60. Kiefer, M., Pulvermüller, F., “Conceptual representations in mind and brain: theoretical developments, current evidence and future directions”, Cortex 48(7), 2012, pp. 805-25.10.1016/j.cortex.2011.04.006Search in Google Scholar

61. Kinsbourne, M., A continuum of self-consciousness that emerges in phylogeny and ontogeny [in:] Herbert S. Terrace and Janet Metcalfe (eds.) The Missing Link in Cognition: Origins of Self-Reflective Consciousness, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 2005.Search in Google Scholar

62. Kiverstein, J., Miller, M., “The embodied brain: towards a radical embodied cognitive neuroscience”, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9, 2015, pp. 237.10.3389/fnhum.2015.00237Search in Google Scholar

63. Klein, S.B., “Making the case that episodic recollection is attributable to operations occurring at retrieval rather than to content stored in a dedicated subsystem of long-term memory”, Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 7, 2013, pp. 3. implications for the self as a construct in psychological research”, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1191, 2010, pp. 1-15.10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00003Search in Google Scholar

65. Koch, Ch., Tsuchiya, N., “Attention and consciousness: two distinct brain processes”, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11(1), 2007, pp. 16-22.10.1016/j.tics.2006.10.012Search in Google Scholar

66. Kriegel, U., “Consciousness and Self-Consciousness”, Monist 87(2), 2004, pp. 182-205.10.5840/monist20048725Search in Google Scholar

67. Lamme, V.A., “Why visual attention and awareness are different”, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7(1), 2003, pp. 12-18.10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00013-XSearch in Google Scholar

68. Lamme, V.A., Roelfsema, P., “The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing”, Trends in Neuroscience 23(11), 2000, pp. 571-579.10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01657-XSearch in Google Scholar

69. Lau, H., Rosenthal, D., “Empirical support for higher-order theories of conscious awareness”, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15(8), 2011, pp. 365-73.10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.00921737339Search in Google Scholar

70. Laureys, S., “The neural correlate of (un)awareness: Lessons from the vegetative state”, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9(12), 2005, pp. 556-559.10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.01016271507Search in Google Scholar

71. Lenggenhager, B., Tadi, T., Metzinger, T., Blanke, O., “Video ergo sum: manipulating bodily self-consciousness”, Science 317(5841), 2007, pp. 1096-1099.Search in Google Scholar

72. Lepsien, J., Nobre, A.C., “Cognitive control of attention in the human brain: insights from orienting attention to mental representations”, Brain Research 1105(1), 2006, pp. 20-31.10.1016/j.brainres.2006.03.03316729979Search in Google Scholar

73. Lombardo, M.V., Chakrabarti, B., Bullmore, E.T.; MRC AIMS Consortium, Baron- Cohen, S., “Specialization of right temporo-parietal junction for mentalizing and its relation to social impairments in autism”, Neuroimage 56(3), 2011, pp. 1832-8.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.06721356316Search in Google Scholar

74. Lombardo, M.V., Chakrabarti, B., Bullmore, E.T., Wheelwright, S.J., Sadek, S.A., Suckling, J.; MRC AIMS Consortium, Baron-Cohen, S., “Shared neural circuits for mentalizing about the self and others”, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22(7), 2009, pp. 1623-1635.10.1162/jocn.2009.2128719580380Search in Google Scholar

75. Mahon, B.Z., Caramazza, A., “A critical look at the embodied cognition hypothesis and a new proposal for grounding conceptual content”, Journal of Physiology-Paris. 102(1-3), 2008, pp. 59-70.10.1016/j.jphysparis.2008.03.00418448316Search in Google Scholar

76. Markowitsch, H.J., “Memory and self-neuroscientific landscapes”, International Scholarly Research Notices Neuroscience 2013, 2013, pp. 176027.10.1155/2013/176027404554024967303Search in Google Scholar

77. Markowitsch HJ, Staniloiu A., “Memory, autonoetic consciousness, and the self”, Consciousness and Cognition 20(1), 2011, pp. 16-39.10.1016/j.concog.2010.09.00520951059Search in Google Scholar

78. Mason, M.F., Norton, M.I., Van Horn, J.D., Wegner, D.M., Grafton, S.T., Macrae, C.N.,, “Wandering minds: the default network and stimulus-independent thought”, Science 315(5810), 2007, pp. 393-395.Search in Google Scholar

79. Mehta, N., Mashour, G.A., “General and specific consciousness: a first-order representationalist approach”, Frontiers in Psychology 4, 2013, pp. 407.10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00407371226923882231Search in Google Scholar

80. Menon, V., Uddin, L.Q., “Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function”, Brain Structure and Function 214(5-6), 2010, pp. 655-667.10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0289988620512370Search in Google Scholar

81. Metzinger, T., Being no one. The self-model theory of subjectivity, Cambridge: MIT Press, 2003.10.7551/mitpress/1551.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

82. Metzinger, T., “Empirical perspectives from the self-model theory of subjectivity: a brief summary with examples”, Progress in Brain Research 168, 2008, pp. 215-245.10.1016/S0079-6123(07)68018-2Search in Google Scholar

83. Miller, S.M., “Closing in on the constitution of consciousness”, Frontiers in Psychology 5, 2014, pp. 1293.10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01293423394525452738Search in Google Scholar

84. Molnar-Szakacs, I., Uddin, L.Q., “Self-processing and the default mode network: interactions with the mirror neuron system”, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 2013, pp. 571.10.3389/fnhum.2013.00571376989224062671Search in Google Scholar

85. Moran, J.M., Kelley, W.M., Heatherton, T.F., “What can the organization of the brain’s default mode network tell us about self-knowledge”, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 2013, pp. 391. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00391371334323882210Search in Google Scholar

86. Musholt, K., “A philosophical perspective on the relation between cortical midline structures and the self”, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 2013, pp. 536.10.3389/fnhum.2013.00536375928324032013Search in Google Scholar

87. Neisser, U., “Five kinds of self-knowledge”. Philosophical Psychology 1, 1988, pp. 35-59.10.1080/09515088808572924Search in Google Scholar

88. Nelson, K., Emerging Levels of Consciousness in Early Human Development [in:] Herbert S. Terrace and Janet Metcalfe (eds.) The Missing Link in Cognition: Origins of Self- Reflective Consciousness, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 2005.Search in Google Scholar

89. Nelson, S.M., Dosenbach, N.U., Cohen, A.L., Wheeler, M.E., Schlaggar, B.L., Petersen, S.E., “Role of the anterior insula in task-level control and focal attention”, Brain Structure and Function 214(5-6), 2010, pp. 669-680.10.1007/s00429-010-0260-2288690820512372Search in Google Scholar

90. Noel, J.P., Pfeiffer, C., Blanke, O., Serino, A., “Peripersonal space as the space of the bodily self”, Cognition 144, 2015, pp. 49-57.10.1016/j.cognition.2015.07.012483789326231086Search in Google Scholar

91. Northoff, G., “From emotions to consciousness - a neuro-phenomenal and neurorelational approach”, Frontiers in Psychology 3, 2012, pp. 303.10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00303343161222969736Search in Google Scholar

92. Northoff, G., “Brain and self - a neurophilosophical account”, Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health 7, 2013, pp. 28.10.1186/1753-2000-7-28373410623902725Search in Google Scholar

93. Northoff, G., Bermpohl, F., “Cortical midline structures and the self”, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8(3), 2004, pp. 102-107.10.1016/j.tics.2004.01.00415301749Search in Google Scholar

94. Olivé, I., Tempelmann, C., Berthoz, A., Heinze, H.J., “Increased functional connectivity between superior colliculus and brain regions implicated in bodily self-consciousness during the rubber hand illusion”, Human Brain Mapping 36(2), 2015, 717-730.10.1002/hbm.22659686938925346407Search in Google Scholar

95. Panksepp, J., Northoff, G., “The trans-species core SELF: the emergence of active cultural and neuro-ecological agents through self-related processing within subcortical-cortical midline networks”, Consciousness and Cognition 18(1), 2009, pp. 193-215.10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.00218485741Search in Google Scholar

96. Parks, E.L., Madden, D.J., “Brain connectivity and visual attention”, Brain Connectivity 3(4), 2013, pp. 317-38.10.1089/brain.2012.0139374970123597177Search in Google Scholar

97. Perlovsky, L., “Language and cognition interaction neural mechanisms”, Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience 2011, 2011, pp. 454587.10.1155/2011/454587316312821876687Search in Google Scholar

98. Perlovsky, L., Ilin, R., “Neurally and mathematically motivated architecture for language and thought, The Open Neuroimaging Journal 4, 2010, pp. 70-80.10.2174/1874440001004020070Search in Google Scholar

99. Pessoa, L., “Understanding brain networks and brain organization”, Physics of Life Reviews 11(3), 2014, pp. 400-35.10.1016/j.plrev.2014.03.005415709924819881Search in Google Scholar

100. Pfeiffer, C., Serino, A., Blanke, O., “The vestibular system: a spatial reference for bodily self-consciousness”, Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 8, 2014, pp. 31.10.3389/fnint.2014.00031402899524860446Search in Google Scholar

101. Posner, M.I., Rothbart, M.K., “Toward a physical basis of attention and self regulation”, Physics of Life Reviews 6(2), 2009, pp. 103-129.10.1016/j.plrev.2009.02.001274894320161073Search in Google Scholar

102. Qin, P., Northoff, G., “How is our self related to midline regions and the defaultmode network?”, Neuroimage 57, 2011, pp. 1221-1233.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.02821609772Search in Google Scholar

103. Qin, P., Wu, X., Huang, Z., Duncan, N.W., Tang, W., Wolff, A., Hu, J., Gao, L., Jin, Y., Wu, X., Zhang, J., Lu, L., Wu, C., Qu, X., Mao, Y., Weng, X., Zhang, J., Northoff, G., “How are different neural networks related to consciousness?”, Annals of Neurology 78(4), 2015, pp. 594-605.10.1002/ana.2447926290126Search in Google Scholar

104. Raffone, A., Pantani, M., “A global workspace model for phenomenal and access consciousness”, Consciousness and Cognition 19(2), 2010, pp. 580-96.10.1016/j.concog.2010.03.01320382038Search in Google Scholar

105. Raffone, A., Srinivasan, N., van Leeuwen, C., “The interplay of attention and consciousness in visual search, attentional blink and working memory consolidation”, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B 369(1641), 2014, pp. 20130215.Search in Google Scholar

106. Raffone, A., Srinivasan, N., van Leeuwen, C., “Rapid switching and complementary evidence accumulation enable flexibility of an all-or-none global workspace for control of attentional and conscious processing: a reply to Wyble et al”, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B 370(1661), 2015, pp. 20140315. Search in Google Scholar

107. Raichle, M.E., MacLeod, A.M., Snyder, A.Z., Powers, W.J., Gusnard, D.A., Shulman, G.L., “A default mode of brain function”, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 98(2), 2001, pp. 676-82.10.1073/pnas.98.2.6761464711209064Search in Google Scholar

108. Rosenthal, D., “Higher-order awareness, misrepresentation and function”, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B 367(1594), 2012, pp. 1424-38.Search in Google Scholar

109. Sala-Llonch, R., Bartres-Faz, D., Junqué C., “Reorganization of brain networks in aging: a review of functional connectivity studies”, Frontiers in Psychology 6, 2015, pp. 663.10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00663443953926052298Search in Google Scholar

110. Sandrone, S., “Self through the mirror (neurons) and default mode network: what neuroscientists found and what can still be found there”, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 2013, pp. 383.10.3389/fnhum.2013.00383372143623898248Search in Google Scholar

111. Schendan, H.E., Ganis, G., “Electrophysiological potentials reveal cortical mechanisms for mental imagery, mental simulation, and grounded (embodied) cognition”, Frontiers in Psychology 3, 2012, pp. 329.10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00329344228123049515Search in Google Scholar

112. Serino, A., Alsmith, A., Costantini, M., Mandrigin, A., Tajadura-Jimenez, A., Lopez, C., “Bodily ownership and self-location: components of bodily self-consciousness”, Consciousness and Cognition 22(4), 2013, pp. 1239-1252.10.1016/j.concog.2013.08.01324025475Search in Google Scholar

113. Shad, M.U., Keshavan, M.S., “Neurobiology of insight deficits in schizophrenia: an fMRI study”, Schizophrenia Research 165(2-3), 2015, pp. 220-226.10.1016/j.schres.2015.04.021445754925957484Search in Google Scholar

114. Shad, M.U., Keshavan, M.S., Steinberg, J.L., Mihalakos, P., Thomas, B.P., Motes, M.A., Soares, J.C., Tamminga, C.A., “Neurobiology of self-awareness in schizophrenia: an fMRI study”, Schizophrenia Research 138(2-3), 2012, pp. 113-119.10.1016/j.schres.2012.03.016337262722480958Search in Google Scholar

115. Singer, T., “The neuronal basis and ontogeny of empathy and mind reading: review of literature and implications for future research”, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 30(6), 2006, pp. 855-863.10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.06.01116904182Search in Google Scholar

116. Soto, D., Silvanto, J., “Reappraising the relationship between working memory and conscious awareness”, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 18(10), 2014, pp. 520-525.10.1016/j.tics.2014.06.00525070269Search in Google Scholar

117. Stawarczyk, D., Majerus, S., Maquet, P., D'Argembeau, A., “Neural correlates of ongoing conscious experience: both task-unrelatedness and stimulus-independence are related to default network activity”, PLoS ONE 6(2), 2011, pp. e16997.10.1371/journal.pone.0016997303893921347270Search in Google Scholar

118. Stevenson, R.J., “Phenomenal and access consciousness in olfaction”, Consciousness and Cognition 18, 2009, pp. 1004-1017.10.1016/j.concog.2009.09.00519815429Search in Google Scholar

119. Sugiura, M., “Associative account of self-cognition: extended forward model and multi-layer structure”, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9, 2013, pp. 535.10.3389/fnhum.2013.00535375732324009578Search in Google Scholar

120. Symons, C.S., Johnson, B.T, “The self-reference effect in memory: a meta-analysis”, Psychological Bulletin 121, 1997, pp. 371-394.10.1037/0033-2909.121.3.3719136641Search in Google Scholar

121. Tagini, A., Raffone, A., “The 'I' and the 'Me' in self-referential awareness: a neurocognitive hypothesis”, Cognitive Processing 11(1), 2010, pp. 9-20.10.1007/s10339-009-0336-119763648Search in Google Scholar

122. Taya, F., Sun, Y., Babiloni, F., Thakor, N., Bezerianos, A., “Brain enhancement through cognitive training: a new insight from brain connectome”, Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience 9, 2015, pp. 44.10.3389/fnsys.2015.00044438164325883555Search in Google Scholar

123. Tulving, E., Elements of episodic memory. New York: Oxford University Press, 1983.Search in Google Scholar

124. Tulving, E., “Memory and consciousness”, Canadian Psychology 26, 1985, pp. 1-12.10.1037/h0080017Search in Google Scholar

125. Tulving, E., “Episodic memory: from mind to brain”, Annual Review of Psychology 53, 2002, pp. 1-25.10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.13511411752477Search in Google Scholar

126. Tulving E., Episodic memory and autonoesis: uniquely human? [in:] Herbert S. Terrace and Janet Metcalfe (eds.) The Missing Link in Cognition: Origins of Self-Reflective Consciousness, Oxford University Press, New York, NY, USA, 2005.Search in Google Scholar

127. van Boxtel, J.J., Tsuchiya, N., Koch, C., “Consciousness and attention: on sufficiency and necessity”, Frontiers in Psychology 1, 2010, pp. 217. 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00217315382221833272Search in Google Scholar

128. Vandekerckhove, M., Bulnes, L.C., Panksepp, J., “The emergence of primary anoetic consciousness in episodic memory”, Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 7, 2014, pp. 210.10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00210387958324427125Search in Google Scholar

129. Vandekerckhove, M., Panksepp, J., “The flow of anoetic to noetic and autonoetic consciousness: a vision of unknowing (anoetic) and knowing (noetic) consciousness in the remembrance of things past and imagined futures”, Consciousness and Cognition 18(4), 2009, pp. 1018-1028.10.1016/j.concog.2009.08.00219713132Search in Google Scholar

130. Vandekerckhove, M., Panksepp, J., “A neurocognitive theory of higher mental emergence: from anoetic affective experiences to noetic knowledge and autonoetic awareness”, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 35(9), 2011, pp. 2017-2025.10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.04.00121530586Search in Google Scholar

131. van der Meer, L., Costafreda, S., Aleman, A., David, A.S., “Self-reflection and the brain: a theoretical review and meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies with implications for schizophrenia”, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 34(6), 2010, pp. 935-946.10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.00420015455Search in Google Scholar

132. Van Essen, D.C., Smith, S.M., Barch, D.M., Behrens, T.E., Yacoub, E., Ugurbil, K.; WU-Minn HCP Consortium, “The WU-Minn Human Connectome Project: an overview”, Neuroimage 80, 2013, 62-79.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.041372434723684880Search in Google Scholar

133. von dem Hagen, E.A., Stoyanova, R.S., Rowe, J.B., Baron-Cohen, S., Calder, A.J., “Direct gaze elicits atypical activation of the theory-of-mind network in autism spectrum conditions”, Cerebral Cortex 24(6), 2014, pp. 1485-92.10.1093/cercor/bht003401418023324559Search in Google Scholar

134. Watanabe, M., “Are there internal thought processes in the monkey?--default brain activity in humans and nonhuman primates”, Behavioural Brain Research 221(1), 2011, pp. 295-303.10.1016/j.bbr.2011.02.03221397636Search in Google Scholar

135. Willems, R.M., Casasanto, D., “Flexibility in embodied language understanding”, Frontiers in Psychology 2, 2011, pp. 116.10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00116313268121779264Search in Google Scholar

136. Wu, X., Zou, Q., Hu, J., Tang, W., Mao, Y., Gao, L., Zhu, J., Jin, Y., Wu, X., Lu, L., Zhang, Y., Zhang, Y., Dai, Z., Gao, J.H., Weng, X., Zhou, L., Northoff, G., Giacino, J.T., He, Y., Yang, Y., “Intrinsic functional connectivity patterns predict consciousness level and recovery outcome in acquired brain injury”, Journal of Neuroscience 35(37), 2015, pp. 12932-46.10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0415-15.2015457161126377477Search in Google Scholar

137. Wyble, B., Bowman, H., Nieuwenstein, M., “On the interplay between working memory consolidation and attentional selection in controlling conscious access: parallel processing at a cost--a comment on 'The interplay of attention and consciousness in visual search, attentional blink and working memory consolidation”, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 370(1661), 2015, pp. 20140197.Search in Google Scholar

138. Zahavi, D., Parnas, J., “Phenomenal consciousness and self-awareness: A phenomenological critique of representational theory”, Journal of Consciousness Studies 5, 1998, pp. 687-705.Search in Google Scholar

139. Zhang, Y., Du, G., Yang, Y., Qin, W., Li, X., Zhang, Q., “Higher integrity of the motor and visual pathways in long-term video game players”, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 9, 2015, pp. 98.10.3389/fnhum.2015.00098435436125805981Search in Google Scholar

eISSN:
2299-0518
Lingua:
Inglese
Frequenza di pubblicazione:
4 volte all'anno
Argomenti della rivista:
Business and Economics, Political Economics, other, Mathematics, Logic and Set Theory, Philosophy