Accès libre

Why Theories of Concepts Should Not Ignore the Problem of Acquisition

   | 31 déc. 2018
À propos de cet article

Citez

Ahn, W., and Kim, N. S. 2000. The role of causal features in categorization: An overview. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 40, ed. by D. L. Medin. New York: Academic Press, 23-65.10.1016/S0079-7421(00)80017-1Search in Google Scholar

Bélanger, J., and Hall, D. G. 2006. Learning proper names and count nouns: Evidence from 16- and 20-month-olds. Journal of Cognition and Development 7, 45-72.10.1207/s15327647jcd0701_3Search in Google Scholar

Bermúdez, J. L. 2007. Thinking without words: An overview for animal ethics. The Journal of Ethics 11(3): 319-335.10.1007/s10892-007-9013-8Open DOISearch in Google Scholar

Block, N. J. 1986. Advertisement for a semantics for psychology. In Midwest studies in philosophy, ed. by P. A. French. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota, 615-678.10.1111/j.1475-4975.1987.tb00558.xSearch in Google Scholar

Braine, M. D. S., and O’Brien, D. P. (Eds.) 1998. Mental Logic. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Search in Google Scholar

Carey, S. 1978. The child as word learner. In Linguistic Theory and Psychological Reality, ed. by J. Bresnan, G. Miller and M. Halle. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 264-293.Search in Google Scholar

Carey, S. 1982. Semantic development, state of the art. In Language Acquisition, State of the Art, ed. by L. Gleitman and E. Wanner. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 347-389.Search in Google Scholar

Carey, S. 1985. Conceptual Change in Childhood. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books, MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar

Carey, S. 2009. The Origin of Concepts. New York: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195367638.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Crain, S. and Khlentzos, D. 2010. The logic instinct. Mind and Language 25(1): 30-65.10.1111/j.1468-0017.2009.01380.xOpen DOISearch in Google Scholar

Dehaene, S. 1997. The Number Sense. New York: Oxford University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Dretske, F. 1981. Knowledge and the Flow of Information. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar

Emlen, S. T. 1975. The stellar-orientation system of a migratory bird. Scientiic American 233: 102-111.10.1038/scientificamerican0875-102Search in Google Scholar

Fodor, J. A. 1975. The Language of Thought. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Fodor, J. A. 1980. On the impossibility of acquiring “more powerful” structures: Fixation of belief and concept acquisition. In Language and Learning, ed. by M. Piatelli-Palmerini. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 142-162.Search in Google Scholar

Fodor, J. A. 1998. Concepts: Where cognitive science went wrong. New York: Oxford University Press.10.1093/0198236360.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Fodor , J. A. 2008. LOT 2: The Language of Thought Revisited. Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199548774.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Fodor, J. A. 2010, October 8. Woof, woof [Review of the book The Origin of Concepts, by S. Carey]. Times Literary Supplement, 7-8.Search in Google Scholar

Frank, M. C., Everett, D. L., Fedorenko, E., and Gibson, E. (in press). Number as a cognitive technology: Evidence from Piraha language and cognition. Cognition 108: 819-824.10.1016/j.cognition.2008.04.007Search in Google Scholar

Gallistel, C. R. 1990. The Organization of Learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar

Gallistel, C. R., Brown, A., Carey, S., Gelman, R., and Keil, F. 1991. Lessons from animal learning for the study of cognitive development. In The Epigenesis of Mind: Essays in Biology and Cognition, ed. by S. Carey and R. Gelman. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 3-36.Search in Google Scholar

Garcia, J., Kimeldorf, D. J., and Koelling, R. A. 1955. Conditioned aversion to saccharin resulting from exposure to gamma radiation. Science 122(3160): 157-8.10.1126/science.122.3160.157Search in Google Scholar

Gelman, R. and Gallistel, C. R. 1978. The Child’s Understanding of Number. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Gelman, S. A. 2003. The Essential Child: Origins of essentialism in everyday thought New York: Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195154061.003.0009Search in Google Scholar

Gordon, P. 2004. Numerical cognition without words: Evidence from Amazonia. Science 306(5695): 496-499.10.1126/science.1094492Search in Google Scholar

Hall, D. G. and Waxman, S. R. 1993. Assumptions about word meaning: Individuation and basic-level kinds. Child Development 64: 1550-1570.10.2307/1131552Open DOISearch in Google Scholar

Hartnett, P., and Gelman, R. 1998. Early understandings of numbers: Paths or barriers to the construction of new understandings? Learning and Instruction 8(4): 341-374.10.1016/S0959-4752(97)00026-1Open DOISearch in Google Scholar

Keil, F. C. 1989. Concepts, Kinds, and Cognitive Development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar

Keleman, D. and Carey, S. 2007. The essence of artifacts: Developing the design stance. In Creations of the Mind: Theories of Artifacts and Their Representation, ed. by E. Margolis and S. Lawrence. New York: Oxford University Press, 212-230.Search in Google Scholar

Kripke, S. 1972/1980. Naming and Necessity. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.10.1007/978-94-010-2557-7_9Search in Google Scholar

Leslie, A. M., Gallistel, C. R., and Gelman, R. 2007. Where integers come from. In The Innate Mind: Foundations and Future, ed. by P. Carruthers, S. Laurence, and S. Stich. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 109-138.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195332834.003.0007Search in Google Scholar

Levin, B. and Pinker, S. 1991. Lexical and Conceptual Semantics. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.1790651Search in Google Scholar

Livingstone, M. S, Srihasam, K., and Morocz, I. A. 2009. The benefit of symbols: monkeys show linear, human-like, accuracy when using symbols to represent scalar value. Animal Cognition 13: 711-719.10.1007/s10071-010-0321-1Open DOISearch in Google Scholar

Macnamara, J. 1986. Border Dispute: The place of logic in psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar

Margolis, E. 1998. How to acquire a concept. Mind and Language 13: 347-369.10.1111/1468-0017.00081Open DOISearch in Google Scholar

Margolis, E. and Laurence, S. 2011. Learning matters: The role of learning in concept acquisition. Mind and Language 26: 507-539.10.1111/j.1468-0017.2011.01429.xOpen DOISearch in Google Scholar

Miller, G. A. 1977. Spontaneous Apprentices: Children and Language. Seabury Press.Search in Google Scholar

Miller, G. A. and Johnson-Laird, P. N. 1976. Language and Perception. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.10.4159/harvard.9780674421288Open DOISearch in Google Scholar

Murphy, G. 2002. The Big Book of Concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/1602.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Penn, D. C., Holyoak, K. J., and Povinelli, D. J. 2008. Darwin’s mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31(2), 109-129.10.1017/S0140525X08003543Open DOISearch in Google Scholar

Pepperberg, I. and Carey, S. 2012. Grey Parrot number acquisition: The inference of cardinal value from ordinal position on the numeral list. Cognition 125: 219-232.10.1016/j.cognition.2012.07.003Search in Google Scholar

Piantadosi, S. T., Tenenbaum, J. B., and Goodman, N. D. 2012. Bootstrapping in a language of thought: a formal model of numerical concept learning. Cognition 123: 199-217.10.1016/j.cognition.2011.11.005Search in Google Scholar

Pica, P., Lemer, C., and Izard, V. 2004. Exact and approximate arithmetic in an Amazonian indigene group. Science 306(5695): 499-503.10.1126/science.1102085Search in Google Scholar

Prasada S,. Ferenz K., and Haskell, T. 2002. Conceiving of entities as objectsand stuff. Cognition 83: 141-165.1186972210.1016/S0010-0277(01)00173-1Search in Google Scholar

Putnam, H. 1975. The meaning of meaning. In Language, Mind, and Knowledge, ed. by K. Gunderson. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 131-193.Search in Google Scholar

Rey, G. 1983. Concepts and stereotypes. Cognition 15: 237-262.10.1016/0010-0277(83)90044-66686511Open DOISearch in Google Scholar

Rey, G. 2014. Innate and learned: Carey, mad dog nativism, and the poverty of stimuli and analogies (yet again). Mind and Language 29: 109-132.10.1111/mila.12044Open DOISearch in Google Scholar

Rips, L. J., Asmuth, J., and Bloomfield, A. 2013. Can statistical learning bootstrap the integers. Cognition 128: 320-330.10.1016/j.cognition.2013.04.001Search in Google Scholar

Rips, L. J., Bloomfield, A., and Asmuth, J. 2008. From numerical concepts to concepts of number. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31: 623-642.10.1017/S0140525X08005566Open DOISearch in Google Scholar

Rips, L. J., and Hespos, S. J. 2011. Rebooting the bootstrap argument: Two puzzles for bootstrap theories of concept development. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34: 145-146.10.1017/S0140525X10002190Search in Google Scholar

Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W., Johnson, D., and Boyes-Braem, P. 1976. Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology 8: 382-439.10.1016/0010-0285(76)90013-XSearch in Google Scholar

Shea, N. 2011. Acquiring a new concept is not explicable-by-content. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34: 148-150.10.1017/S0140525X10002219Open DOISearch in Google Scholar

Smith, C., Carey, S., and Wiser, M. 1985. On differentiation: a case study of the development of size, weight, and density. Cognition 21(3): 177-237.10.1016/0010-0277(85)90025-33830547Open DOISearch in Google Scholar

Smith, E., and Medin, D. 1981. Categories and Concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.10.4159/harvard.9780674866270Search in Google Scholar

Spaepen, E., Coppola, M., Spelke. E, Carey, S., and Goldin-Meadow, S. 2011. Number without a language model. PNAS 108(8): 3163-8.10.1073/pnas.1015975108Search in Google Scholar

Strevens, M. 2000. The essentialist aspect of naïve theories. Cognition 74: 149-175.10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00071-210617780Open DOISearch in Google Scholar

Strevens, M. 2009. Theoretical terms without analytic truths. Philosophical Studies 143: 91-100.10.1007/s11098-008-9312-3Search in Google Scholar

Weiskopf, D. 2008. The origins of concepts. Philosophical Studies 140: 359-384.10.1007/s11098-007-9150-8Search in Google Scholar

Wiser, M. and Carey, S. 1983. When heat and temperature were one. In Mental Models, ed. by D. Gentner and A. Stevens. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 267-297.Search in Google Scholar

eISSN:
0873-626X
Langues:
Anglais, Portuguese
Périodicité:
4 fois par an
Sujets de la revue:
Philosophy, Selected Philosophical Movements, Analytical Philosophy