Uneingeschränkter Zugang

Thinking Through Tools: What Can Tool-Use Tell Us About Distributed Cognition?

   | 24. Juli 2015

Zitieren

Baber, C. (2003). Cognition and tool use: Forms of engagement in animal and human tool use. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.Search in Google Scholar

Baber, C. (2006). Cognitive aspects of tool use. Applied Ergonomics, 37, 3–15.10.1016/j.apergo.2005.06.004Search in Google Scholar

Baber, C., Cengiz, T. G., & Parekh, M. (2014). Tool use as distributed cognition: How tools help, hinder and define manual skill. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 116, 1–14.10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00116Search in Google Scholar

Baber, C., Cengiz, T. G., Starke, S., & Parekh, M., (2015a). Objective classification of performance in the use of a piercing saw in jewellery making. Applied Ergonomics, 51, 211–221.10.1016/j.apergo.2015.05.002Search in Google Scholar

Baber, C., Starke, S., & Cengiz, T. G. (2015b). Using 1/f scaling to study variability and dexterity in simple tool using tasks. Proceedings of the 59th Annual Meeting of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society. Santa Monica, CA: HFES.10.1177/1541931215591091Search in Google Scholar

Bernstein, N. A. (1967). The coordination and regulation of movements. Oxford: Pergamon Press.Search in Google Scholar

Biryukova, E. V., & Bril, B. (2008). Organization of goaldirected action at a high-level of motor skill: The case of stone-knapping in India. Motor Control, 12, 181–209.10.1123/mcj.12.3.181Search in Google Scholar

Bril, B., Rein, R., Nonaka, T., Wenban-Smith, F., & Dietrich, G. (2010). The role of expertise in tool use: Skill differences in functional action adaptations to task constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 36, 825–839.Search in Google Scholar

Chao, L. L., & Martin, A. (2000). Representation of manipulable man-made objects in the dorsal stream. Neuroimage, 12, 478–484.10.1006/nimg.2000.0635Search in Google Scholar

Chemero, A. (2009). Radical embodied cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.10.7551/mitpress/8367.001.0001Search in Google Scholar

Cutler, A. (1994). The hand of the master: craftsmanship, ivory and society in Byzantium. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Search in Google Scholar

Grafton, S. T., Fadiga, L., Arbib, M. A., & Rizzolatti, G. (1997). Premotor cortex activation during observation and naming of familiar tools. Neuoroimage, 6, 231–236.10.1006/nimg.1997.0293Search in Google Scholar

Guastello, S. J. (2000). Nonlinear dynamics in psychology. Discrete Dynamics in Nature and Society, 6, 11–29.Search in Google Scholar

Higuchi, S., Imamizu, H., & Kawato, M. (2007). Cerebellar activity evoked by common tool-use execution and imagery tasks: an fMRI study. Cortex, 43, 350–358.10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70460-XSearch in Google Scholar

Ingvaldsen, R. P., & Whiting, H. T. A. (1997). Modern views on motor skill learning are not ‘representative’ ! Human Movement Science, 16, 705–732.10.1016/S0167-9457(97)00022-5Search in Google Scholar

Johnson-Frey, S. H. (2004). The neural bases of complex tool use in humans. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 71–78.10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.002Search in Google Scholar

Kamm K., Thelen E., & Jensen J. L. (1990). A dynamical systems approach to motor development. Physical Therapy, 70, 763–775.10.1093/ptj/70.12.763Search in Google Scholar

Kello, C. T., Beltz, B. C., Holden, J. G., & van Orden, G. C. (2007). The emergent coordination of cognitive function. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 551–568.10.1037/0096-3445.136.4.551Search in Google Scholar

Kelso, J. A. S. (1995). Dynamic patterns: The self-organization of brain and behavior. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Search in Google Scholar

Krist, V. (2012). The cognitive bases of human tool use. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 203–218.10.1017/S0140525X11001452Search in Google Scholar

Latash M. L., Scholz, J. P., & Schöner, G. (2002). Motor control strategies revealed in the structure of motor variability. Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews, 30, 26–31.10.1097/00003677-200201000-00006Search in Google Scholar

Martin, A. (2007). The representation of object concepts in the brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 25–45.10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190143Search in Google Scholar

Mitra, S., Riley, M. A., & Turvey, M. T. (1997). Chaos in human rhythmic movement. Journal of Motor Behavior, 29, 195–198.10.1080/00222899709600834Search in Google Scholar

Newell K. M., & Corcos, D. M. (1993). Issues in variability and motor control. In K. M. Newell & D. M. Corcos (Eds.), Variability and motor control (pp. 1–12). Champagne, IL: Human Kinetics.Search in Google Scholar

Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action. New York: Springer.Search in Google Scholar

Parry, R., Dietrich, G., & Bril, B. (2014). Tool use ability depends on understanding of functional dynamics and not specific joint coordination profiles. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 306, 1–15.Search in Google Scholar

Pye, D. (1968). The nature and art of workmanship. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Search in Google Scholar

Rein, R., Bril, B., & Nonaka, T. (2013). Coordination strategies used in stone knapping. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 150, 539–550.10.1002/ajpa.22224Search in Google Scholar

Rosenbaum, D. A. (1980). Human movement initiation: Specification of aim, direction, and extent. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109, 444–474.10.1037/0096-3445.109.4.444Search in Google Scholar

Roux, V., Bril, B., & Dietrich, G. (1995). Skills and learning difficulties involved in stone knapping: The case of stone-bead knapping in Khambhat, India. World Archaeology, 27, 63–87.10.1080/00438243.1995.9980293Search in Google Scholar

Schmidt, R. A. (2003). Motor schema theory after 27 years: Reflections and implications for a new theory. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 74, 366–375.10.1080/02701367.2003.10609106Search in Google Scholar

Summers, J. J., & Anson, J. G. (2009). Current status of the motor program: Revisited. Human Movement Science, 28(5), 566–577.10.1016/j.humov.2009.01.002Search in Google Scholar

Thelen E. (1990). Motor development: a new synthesis. American Psychologist, 50, 79–95.10.1037/0003-066X.50.2.79Search in Google Scholar

Turvey, M. (1990). Coordination. American Psychologist, 45, 938–953.10.1037/0003-066X.45.8.938Search in Google Scholar

Van Leeuwen, L., Smitsman, A., & van Leeuwen, C. (1994). Affordances, perceptual complexity, and the development of tool use. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(1), 174–191.Search in Google Scholar

Van Orden, G. C., Holden, J. G., & Turvey, M. T. (2003). Self-organization of cognitive performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 331–350.10.1037/0096-3445.132.3.331Search in Google Scholar

Wagenmakers, E.-J., Farrell, S., & Ratcliff, R. (2004). Estimation and interpretation of l/f alpha noise in human cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 579–615.10.3758/BF03196615Search in Google Scholar

Weisberg, J., van Turennout, M., & Martin, A. (2007). A neural system for learning about object function. Cerebral Cortex, 17(3), 513–521.10.1093/cercor/bhj176Search in Google Scholar

eISSN:
2199-6059
ISSN:
0860-150X
Sprache:
Englisch
Zeitrahmen der Veröffentlichung:
4 Hefte pro Jahr
Fachgebiete der Zeitschrift:
Philosophie, andere