1. bookVolume 76 (2021): Edizione 1 (January 2021)
Dettagli della rivista
License
Formato
Rivista
eISSN
1899-7562
Prima pubblicazione
13 Jan 2009
Frequenza di pubblicazione
5 volte all'anno
Lingue
Inglese
Accesso libero

Synergistic Activation Patterns of Hand Muscles in Left-and Right-Hand Dominant Individuals

Pubblicato online: 29 Jan 2021
Volume & Edizione: Volume 76 (2021) - Edizione 1 (January 2021)
Pagine: 89 - 100
Dettagli della rivista
License
Formato
Rivista
eISSN
1899-7562
Prima pubblicazione
13 Jan 2009
Frequenza di pubblicazione
5 volte all'anno
Lingue
Inglese
Introduction

Most individuals show a preference for use of one hand or the other for both activities of daily living and specialized skills such as playing an instrument. Skill training for sport is often highly lateralized to the preferred hand, especially for those skills which require a high degree of fine motor control such as dart throwing, pitching, and shooting. Traditionally, inventories of hand preference for a set of tasks have been used to define handedness or hand dominance (Bryden, 1977; Oldfield, 1971). Handedness inventories indicate about 90% of the population is right-hand dominant, and left-hand dominant individuals indicate preference for the non-dominant arm more frequently than right-hand dominant individuals (Borod et al., 1984). A traditional view, a global dominance hypothesis, which emerged from much of this work, posited that the dominant arm is superior to the non-dominant arm in all aspects of control. However, more thorough investigation of inter-limb differences in motor behavior has led to a competing hypothesis that the dominant and non-dominant limb may show different specialization (Woytowicz et al., 2018). The dynamic-dominance hypothesis of motor lateralization (Sainburg, 2002), suggests that in right-hand dominant individuals the dominant right arm is specialized for predictive control of dynamics to produce smooth and efficient movements, whereas the non-dominant left arm is specialized for stability to unanticipated perturbations (Sainburg, 2005). Subsequent work showed the dynamic dominance hypothesis holds for left-hand dominant individuals, however limb specialization asymmetry was decreased primarily due to greater predictive control of dynamics on the non-dominant arm than is typical in right-hand dominant individuals (Przybyla et al., 2012). There is now a wealth of behavioral evidence in support of the dynamic-dominance hypothesis (for review, Sainburg, 2014), however, the neural underpinnings of motor lateralization and handedness are less clear (Andersen and Siebner, 2018).

The investigation of modularity in muscle co-activation has aided our understanding of motor control and motor pathology (Andersen and Siebner, 2018), yet has been sparsely used in the study of handedness. A long-standing hypothesis in motor control questions whether dexterous coordination of a musculoskeletal system with numerous redundant degrees of freedom (Bernstein, 1967; Latash, 2012; Tresch and Jarc, 2009; Tresch et al., 1999) may be simplified through the co-activation of specific groups of muscles that form basis sets, commonly referred to as muscle synergies, from which complex actions can be assembled (for review, Bizzi and Cheung, 2013; Bizzi et al., 1991; Tresch and Jarc, 2009). Though criticisms of this thesis have been put forward, such as for example that synergies may be artifacts of the musculoskeletal constraints of the limb or biomechanical constraints of the task (Kutch and Valero-Cuevas, 2012), the use of muscle synergies to describe and compare fixed regularities in muscle activation remains a valuable tool for the study of motor control and has been widely applied across species and tasks.

The limited research testing for differences in synergistic co-activation of muscles of the upper limb, as a function of handedness, have primarily focused on the shoulder and elbow during whole arm movements. In the most rigorous comparison of dominant and non-dominant upper limb synergies to date, left and right arm shoulder and elbow muscle synergies were extracted during a center out reaching task under different load conditions in 41 right-handed healthy individuals (Pellegrino et al., 2020). Results indicated a high degree of synergy similarity between the limbs across all force conditions, despite significant differences in muscle activation, suggesting differences in the activation of motor modules but invariance in module structure. In a second study, inter-limb differences in shoulder and elbow muscle synergies were extracted during a whole arm isometric force task in healthy right handed individuals, as control comparison to assess differences in the affected and unaffected limbs of individuals with hemiparesis due to stroke (Roh et al., 2013). Results indicated a high degree of similarity not only between the right and left limb, but also between participants in the sample. In a study that included both left- and right-hand dominant individuals, Duthilleul and coworkers (Duthilleul et al., 2015) observed that the number of upper limb muscle synergies observed during circular arm movements were similar between dominant and non-dominant limbs but different between left- and right-hand dominant individuals for gross movements (wide circles). In contrast, finer movements (smaller circles) were associated with higher number of synergies for dominant arm, but no difference between left- and right- handed individuals. Notably, this is the only study to include muscles of the forearm in analysis.

The above-mentioned work has set a foundation for understanding patterns of muscle co-activation associated with handedness in the proximal upper limb. However, it is well-known that the corticospinal control of the proximal upper limb is different from that of the distal upper limb. For example, there is a greater density of minimally divergent monosynaptic corticospinal projections to the alpha motor neurons that innervate the intrinsic hand muscles compared to those alpha motor neurons that innervate the proximal muscles of the upper arm (Rathelot and Strick, 2006; Rathelot and Strick, 2009; Zaaimi et al., 2018). Therefore, it cannot be assumed that findings relating muscle synergies and handedness for the proximal limb would also be found in the distal limb and hand. In this investigation we specifically tested the similarity of hand muscle synergies between the non-dominant and dominant arm of right-handed and left-handed individuals. EMG data were extracted from voluntary movements in the American Sign Language posture set. This task provides wide variability of hand postures and finger individuation and has previously been used for hand synergy investigation (Ajiboye and Weir, 2009). Comparisons of synergies were made between the limbs of each individual to test for differences in inter-limb synergy similarity between left- and right-hand dominant individuals, and between limbs of all individuals in order to describe limb-specific synergy similarity across the sample population.

Methods
Participants

Ten young healthy participants with no known history of neurological disease participated following informed consent in accordance with the Institutional Review Board of Northeastern University. Five participants were right-hand dominant (27.8 ±8.0 years old) and five participants were left-hand dominant (22.8 ±3.6 years old) according to the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971).

Electromyography

Surface EMG (Trigno, Delsys Inc. Natick, MA, USA) was recorded from 15 muscles: 1st dorsal interosseus (FDI), extensor indicus (EI), 3rd dorsal interosseous (3DI), 3rd lumbrical (3LUM), adductor pollicis brevis (ADPB), abductor pollicis brevis (ABPB), abductor digiti minimi (ADM), flexor digiti minimi (FDM), flexor carpi radialis (FCR), flexor carpi ulnaris (FCU), flexor digitorum superficialis (FDS), extensor digitorum (EDC), and extensor carpi radialis (ECR), extensor carpi ulnaris (ECU), brachioradialis (BRD). All EMG data were sampled at 1000Hz.

Experimental Task

Data were acquired for the right and left limbs in two separate sessions, less than one week apart. Participants were seated with the elbow supported and wrist free to move. In each session, participants shaped their hand into letters and numbers of the American Sign Language (ASL) posture set (Fig. 1) presented as pictures on a computer screen (33 postures, 3 repetitions, 99 trials). Dynamic letters ‘J’ and ‘Z’ were omitted, along with the number “0”, which is visually the same as the letter “O”. For each character, participants were given two seconds to form the posture, six seconds to maintain it, and two seconds to return to the relaxed starting posture. Participants were instructed to form each pose with sufficient force to maintain the posture as if they were signing to someone seated across from them. Participants were specifically instructed to limit motion at the elbow during posturing. All participants were naive to ASL prior to participation.

Figure 1

Experimental setup. Left. The participant reproduces the ALS posture shown on the computer screen. Right. EMGs acquired during the production of three hand postures in the ASL alphabet are presented. The shaded regions indicate the window used for analysis.

EMG Processing & Synergy Extraction

All data processing was performed offline using custom analyses in Matlab (Mathworks Inc.). EMG data (size m, muscles) from each ASL pose (p) was filtered with a zero phase fourth order Butterworth bandpass filter with cutoffs at 10Hz and 300Hz. The mean absolute value (MAV) of the filtered EMG within a 2 second time window (5.5 – 7.5 sec from trial start) was determined for each muscle and normalized to the corresponding muscle’s within trial maximum activation. The time window for MAV extraction was chosen to capture steady state activation associated with maintenance of the pose. Data for each pose across all trials were concatenated into a matrix [size: muscles (15) x poses (99)] for each hand individually.

Non-negative matrix factorization (Lee and Seung, 1999) was used to extract muscle synergies and their corresponding activation coefficients from the recorded muscle activity. For a preselected rank (N) representing the number of synergies to be included, EMG matrices (M) were factorized into a synergy matrix (B, size m x p) containing a linear combination of time invariant non-negative vectors and an activation matrix (A, size p x N) containing the pose-dependent nonnegative activation coefficients of each synergy (Equation 1).

M=BAε$$ \begin{equation} M=B A-\varepsilon \end{equation}$$

The extracted synergies (B) reflect the spatially fixed regularities (Kargo and Giszter, 2008; Safavynia and Ting, 2012) in muscle groupings that are common across the formation of different poses. NMF finds B and A by minimizing the squared norm of the residual (ε), under the assumption that it follows a Gaussian distribution that is zero mean and uncorrelated. The algorithm iteratively updates the model parameters until the squared norm of the residual changes by less than 0.001 over ten iterations.

To select the rank (N) to be used in subsequent analysis, the NMF process was repeated 10,000 times (in order to increase the likelihood of the NMF approximating the global minima) for each possible number of synergies 1 to 15 (the number of muscles examined).

R2=1SSESST=Mi,j(BA)i,jMi,jM¯2$$R^{2}=1-\frac{SSE}{SST}=\frac{\sum\left(M_{i, j}-(B A)_{i, j}\right.}{\sum\left(M_{i, j}-\bar{M}\right)^{2}}$$

For the best fit run at each rank, the proportion of variance explained by the model (R2) was determined as described in Equation 2, where SSE is the sum of squared error, SST is the total sum of squares, subscripts (i,j) denote the corresponding entry of M, and is the average over all entries of M (Devarajan and Cheung, 2014). Rank N for subsequent analysis was chosen as the minimum number of synergies needed to reconstruct 90% of the variance in the data from which they were derived (R2 ≥ .90). This methodology has been used extensively for the purpose of deriving a low dimensional representation of synergistic muscle activation (Duthilleul et al., 2015; Sawers et al., 2015; Tresch et al., 2006; Tresch and Jarc, 2009), and has been described by our group previously (Günay et al., 2019; Jacobs-Skolik et al., 2020).

Quantification of Similarity Between Synergies

To compare individual synergies between different hands, dot products were computed between all possible pairs of synergies (one from each hand), and a greedy search procedure was used to define matching pairs. The best-matching pair was defined as the one with the highest dot product; this pair was removed, and the next best matching pair was selected as the highest dot product among the remaining synergy pairs. This process continued until there were no more unpaired synergies left in the set of lower rank (Tresch et al., 1999). If rank differed between sets, the set with the higher rank was left with an unmatched synergy(ies). A similarity index (SI) describing the global similarity in synergistic muscle activation was determined by averaging the dot products for the chosen pairings.

Statistical Design

To assess differences in the number of synergies required to achieve R2 ≥ .90 between hands [Right Dominant Right Hand (RD-RH), Right Dominant Left Hand (RD-LH), Left Dominant Right Hand (LD-RH), Left Dominant Left Hand (LD-LH)], we conducted a 2-way ANOVA with factors of Dominance (RD, LD) and Side (RH, LH). To compare synergy similarity of the right hand and left hand (RH & LH pairs) between RD and LD participants, an independent samples t-test was used to test for differences in SI. To assess differences in synergy similarity associated with side or dominance between RD and LD individuals, we determined the SI of synergy pairs made from each hand of LD participants to each hand of RD participants. Separate independent samples t-tests were used to test for differences in SI between right (RD-RH & LD-RH) and left hands (RD-LH & LD-LH), and between dominant (RD-RH & LD-LH) and non-dominant hands (RD-LH & LD-RH). Each comparison group (i.e. RD-RH & LD-RH) contained values from 25 hand pairs. Similarity of hand specific synergies across participants was determined by obtaining SI values of synergy pairs made from matching all hands of the same type (i.e. every RD-RH hand to every other RD-RH). This procedure yielded a total of 10 unique pairs excluding pairing any hand to itself. Differences in the similarity of hand specific synergies (RD-RH, RD-LH, LD-RH, LD-LH) were tested using a 2-way ANOVA with factors of Dominance (RD, LD) and Side (RH, LH). Alpha values for all tests were set to .05. Post-hoc tests with correction for multiple comparisons were applied where main effects or interactions were found to be significant. All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS (version 20; SPSS, Chicago, IL).

Results

Across all participants and all arms 8.7 ±1.0 (range: 8-11) synergies were required to reconstruct the original EMG data at R2 ≥ .90 (Table 1, Fig. 2). On average a greater number of synergies were observed for LD participants, however a 2-way ANOVA with factors Hand (RH, LH) and Dominance (RD, LD) indicated no significant main effect of Hand (F1,16 = 3.459, p = .081), or Dominance (F1,16 = 0.00, p = 1.000), and no Hand x Dominance interaction (F1,16 = 0.216, p = .648).

Figure 2

Left. Participant mean (±sd) reconstruction (R2) for each hand for ranks (1-15). R2 increases monotonically with increased rank. Right. Number of muscle synergies required to reconstruct task EMG data satisfying R2 ≥ .9.

Number of muscle synergies required to reconstruct task EMG data satisfying R2 ≥ .9 for each hand tested.

S1 - LDS2 - LDS3 - LDS4 - LDS5 - LDMean
LH11881099.2
RH1098999
S6 - RDS7 - RDS8 - RDS9 - RDS10 - RD
LH1088878.2
RH888998.4

Matched pairs of LH & RH synergies for an example left-hand dominant participant and right-hand dominant participant are shown in Figure 3. The SI between LH & RH synergies for LD participants was .77 ± .05 (range: .70 - .83), and for RD participants was .74 ± .06 (range: .66 - .83). No significant difference in SI of the RH & LH was observed between LD and RD participants (t8 = .660, p = .528).

Figure 3

Greedy search matched left hand (black) and right hand (gray) synergy pairs for a left hand dominant (LD, top, participant 3), and a right hand dominant (RD, bottom, participant 8) participant. Numbers above or below synergy pairs indicate the pairwise dot product.

Specific comparisons of similarity index (SI) between LD and RD participants were used to assess differences in synergy similarity between the left-hand (LD-LH & RD-LH) and right-hand (LD-RH & RD-RH), and differences in synergy similarity between dominant (LD-LH & RD-RH) and non-dominant hand (LD-RH & RD-LH). SI of left hands (.75 ± .05, range: .63 -.85) was significantly greater (t8 = 2.85, p = .006) than SI of right hands (.70 ± .05, range: .60 -.80). In contrast, SI of non-dominant hands (.72 ± .05, range: .66 - .89) was not significantly different (t8 = 2.85, p=.006) than SI of dominant hands (72 ± .06, range: .62 -.82), see Fig. 4.

Figure 4

Similarity indexes showing comparison between every arm used in the study. Hotter colors indicate a higher similarity index signifying greater similarity of synergies between paired arms. Boxes indicate group comparisons. Within arm analysis of LD-LH is highlighted in blue to draw attention to the comparatively high similarity of limbs within this grouping.

Hand specific SI across participants was found for LD-LH (.79 ± .06, range: .73 -.88), LD-RH (.71 ± .03, range: .68 -.75), RD-LH (.73 ± .05, range: .65 -.80), and RD-RH (.69 ± .06, range: .62 - .77). A 2-way ANOVA to test for differences in hand specific SI indicated a significant main effect of Hand (F1,36 = 13.746, p = .001), and a significant main effect of Dominance (F1,36 = 10.055, p = .003), but no Hand x Dominance interaction (F1,36 = 1.815, p = .186). Post-hoc independent samples t-tests with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons (.05/6 = .008) indicated significant differences in population level synergy similarity between LD-LH & RD-RH (t18 = -4.112, p < .001) and between LD-LH & LD-RH (t18 = -3.882, P = .001), and a trend level difference between LD-LH & RD-LH (t18 = -2.963, P = .008).

Discussion

In this study we examined whether limb dominance and side are associated with specific temporally invariant patterns of muscle co-activation during volitional movement. Unlike prior investigations which focused on the proximal upper limb (Duthilleul et al., 2015; Pellegrino et al., 2020; Roh et al., 2013), we focused on the intrinsic and extrinsic hand muscles of the distal upper limb and used a task that required a relatively high degree of finger individuation.

Our data reveals two main findings. First, that similarity of hand muscle synergies among left hands (across LD and RD individuals) was greater than that of right hands. Second, synergy similarity in the sample population was greater for the LD-LH case than the other hands tested (LD-RH, RD-LH, RD-RH). Unfortunately, only one of the three previous studies that compared synergies between the limbs tested both LD and RD individuals, limiting our ability to make direct comparisons to prior work. Duthilleul and colleagues (Duthilleul et al., 2015) reported two main findings regarding synergy composition between the limbs as a function of handedness. For gross movements of the arm (wide circle drawing), LD individuals showed greater synergy complexity (more synergies) than RD individuals, though no difference in synergy complexity was noted between the dominant and non-dominant limbs. In contrast, for finer-grained movements (small circles) the LD-LH showed lower complexity than other arms tested. These results are in contrast to the results of the present study where a greater, though not statistically significant, number of synergies was observed for the LD-LH condition compared to other hands, perhaps indicating differences between proximal and distal LD-LH synergies. Despite this difference, our study and that of Duthilleul both seem to depart from evidence suggesting similar behavior of the dominant hand in LD and RD individuals, and less asymmetry of limb specialization in LD individuals (Dassonville et al., 1997; Oldfield, 1971; Przybyla et al., 2012).

Quantitative analysis of limb kinematics in LD individuals has shown that reduced limb asymmetry is primarily due to greater dominant limb-like behavior of the non-dominant limb (Przybyla et al., 2012). It has been suggested that greater recruitment of ipsilateral motor cortex resulting in more symmetrical activation of the hemispheres, may be the neurological substrate of reduced behavioral asymmetry in left hand dominant individuals (Dassonville et al., 1997). However, whether these results reflect a consequence or cause of handedness remains an active research topic (Andersen and Siebner, 2018). Furthermore, we know of no study that has specifically looked at population variability of limb-specific behavior or hemispheric activation asymmetry in left- and right-handed individuals. We therefore offer a speculative explanation as to why in this study LD-LH synergies appeared to be more invariant in the population than synergies of other limbs. One possibility is that population invariance of LD-LH synergies results from the demands imposed on left-hand dominant individuals to use their left hands in the context of environments designed mainly for right-handed users. Given that most objects we interact with require right-handed manipulation, the left hand of left-hand dominant individuals, despite being specialized for predictive control per the dynamic-dominance hypothesis, may be forced into a stabilization role during bimanual activities, allowing the right hand to manipulate the object (Przybyla et al., 2012). Extended practice in a motor skill, for example a musical instrument, can shape muscle co-activation patterns, in essence creating use-dependent specialization of synergies (Gentner et al., 2010). It is possible that more varied use of the left hand by left-hand dominant individuals manifests in a more generic set of synergies than is seen in other limb-dominance combinations, thereby explaining greater similarity of LD-LH synergies within the population. However, the existence of a causal relationship between use-dependent neural reorganization and greater symmetry of hemispheric activation is still unknown (Andersen and Siebner, 2018).

As previously stated, our results did not reveal significant differences between the non-dominant and dominant limb at the individual or population level, in contrast to what might be expected from kinematic analyses showing specialized roles for the dominant and non-dominant limb as postulated in the dynamic dominance hypothesis (Sainburg, 2002; Sainburg, 2005). This result is in general agreement with previous investigations of the inter-limb synergy similarity of proximal upper limb muscles (Pellegrino et al., 2020; Roh et al., 2013), despite several reasons to suspect differences between the proximal and distal limb. Foremost, the degree to which the dynamic dominance hypothesis, which has mainly been predicated on experiments involving reach movements, holds for movement of the fingers remains unclear. A study of isometric finger force production and force stabilization supports the idea that the dominant limb may be specialized for adjustments to performance variables, however no evidence was found for non-dominant specialization for stabilizing steady-state isometric forces, as would be predicted by dynamic dominance (Zhang et al., 2006). While conclusions from a study of isometric force production may not be transferable to movements such as those used in the present study, the study by (Zhang et al., 2006) provides grounds for likely differences in inter-limb synergy similarity between proximal and distal limb segments.

Differences in the neurophysiological substrates of proximal and distal limb control, when viewed in the context of cortical differences associated with motor lateralization and handedness, provide a second reason to hypothesize that inter-limb synergy similarity may differ between the upper-arm and hand. It is well established that the alpha motor neurons innervating the distal upper-limb and hand receive a greater density of less divergent, monosynaptic, corticospinal projections than those synapsing onto motor neurons controlling the proximal limb, which often involve spinal interneurons (Rathelot and Strick, 2006; Rathelot and Strick, 2009; Zaaimi et al., 2018). Though some evidence in support of cortical synergies exists (Leo et al., 2016), it is spinal interneurons that have been consistently identified as an origin of synergistic muscle activation (Bizzi and Cheung, 2013; Hart and Giszter, 2010). However, differences in spinal anatomy and physiology are sparsely discussed in relation to handedness, and handedness is generally approached in the literature as something that is cortical in origin (Andersen and Siebner, 2018). Indeed, evidence indicating that fine motor control of the hand is dependent on direct projections from the contralateral motor cortex (M1) to spinal motorneurons (Lawrence and Kuypers, 1968; Muir and Lemon, 1983) has fueled hypotheses that handedness is reflected in the functional and structural hemispheric asymmetries in M1 (for a review, Andersen and Siebner, 2018; Goble and Brown, 2008; Hammond, 2002). Investigations have typically focused on handedness-related asymmetries in M1 structure (Amunts et al., 1996), functional topography (Triggs et al., 1999; Volkmann et al., 1998), activation during unilateral movement (Dassonville et al., 1997; Solodkin et al., 2001), cortical excitability (Brouwer et al., 2001; Ziemann and Hallett, 2001), functional connectivity (Pool et al., 2014), and effects of lesion due to stroke (Mani et al., 2014; Schaefer et al., 2009; Schaefer et al., 2012). Pellegrino and coauthors (Pellegrino et al., 2020) suggest the reason that differences in proximal upper-limb muscle synergies between arms were found only for synergy activation coefficients but not in number and structure of the muscle synergies themselves, is that the synergies observed are best attributed to muscle activation by spinal interneuron circuits. They do speculate that this finding may not hold for fine movements of the hand due to more direct cortical control. However, we did not find evidence that such a difference exists between dominant and non-dominant limb.

Several factors limit the interpretation of results in this study. First, the study was limited by a small sample size, though it was comparable to that of (Duthilleul et al., 2015). All participants reported to be naïve to ASL, however no history was taken on other forms of specialized mass practice with the hand, such as dart throwing, video gaming, or musical instrument playing. However, if such extreme skill specialization was present in our sample, it would have likely reflected as an outlier in the data, which was not the case. Finally, only a single task was used; therefore, it remains unknown whether synergies observed here were specific to the task or generalizable to a broader repertoire of hand function.

Here we investigated dexterous but not skilled movements. It has been suggested that the acquisition of new motor skills may involve the development of new muscle synergies, merging or fractionation of current synergies, or change their temporal activation (Asaka et al., 2008; Cheung et al., 2020; Frere and Hug, 2012). We hypothesize that a greater difference in muscle synergies between the dominant and non-dominant hand than was observed in this study may exist for individuals whose athletic training has created a large limb asymmetry for a highly specialized skill. Further examination into hand muscle synergies of left- and right-hand dominant individuals could improve training methods especially in sports that have high demands on precision in fine motor skills such as dart throwing, pitching, or e-sports where milliseconds can be the difference between a win or loss.

Figure 1

Experimental setup. Left. The participant reproduces the ALS posture shown on the computer screen. Right. EMGs acquired during the production of three hand postures in the ASL alphabet are presented. The shaded regions indicate the window used for analysis.
Experimental setup. Left. The participant reproduces the ALS posture shown on the computer screen. Right. EMGs acquired during the production of three hand postures in the ASL alphabet are presented. The shaded regions indicate the window used for analysis.

Figure 2

Left. Participant mean (±sd) reconstruction (R2) for each hand for ranks (1-15). R2 increases monotonically with increased rank. Right. Number of muscle synergies required to reconstruct task EMG data satisfying R2 ≥ .9.
Left. Participant mean (±sd) reconstruction (R2) for each hand for ranks (1-15). R2 increases monotonically with increased rank. Right. Number of muscle synergies required to reconstruct task EMG data satisfying R2 ≥ .9.

Figure 3

Greedy search matched left hand (black) and right hand (gray) synergy pairs for a left hand dominant (LD, top, participant 3), and a right hand dominant (RD, bottom, participant 8) participant. Numbers above or below synergy pairs indicate the pairwise dot product.
Greedy search matched left hand (black) and right hand (gray) synergy pairs for a left hand dominant (LD, top, participant 3), and a right hand dominant (RD, bottom, participant 8) participant. Numbers above or below synergy pairs indicate the pairwise dot product.

Figure 4

Similarity indexes showing comparison between every arm used in the study. Hotter colors indicate a higher similarity index signifying greater similarity of synergies between paired arms. Boxes indicate group comparisons. Within arm analysis of LD-LH is highlighted in blue to draw attention to the comparatively high similarity of limbs within this grouping.
Similarity indexes showing comparison between every arm used in the study. Hotter colors indicate a higher similarity index signifying greater similarity of synergies between paired arms. Boxes indicate group comparisons. Within arm analysis of LD-LH is highlighted in blue to draw attention to the comparatively high similarity of limbs within this grouping.

Number of muscle synergies required to reconstruct task EMG data satisfying R2 ≥ .9 for each hand tested.

S1 - LDS2 - LDS3 - LDS4 - LDS5 - LDMean
LH11881099.2
RH1098999
S6 - RDS7 - RDS8 - RDS9 - RDS10 - RD
LH1088878.2
RH888998.4

Ajiboye AB, Weir RF. Muscle synergies as a predictive framework for the EMG patterns of new hand postures. J Neural Eng 2009; 6: 036004AjiboyeABWeirRFMuscle synergies as a predictive framework for the EMG patterns of new hand posturesJ Neural Eng2009603600410.1088/1741-2560/6/3/036004Search in Google Scholar

Amunts K, Schlaug G, Schleicher A, Steinmetz H, Dabringhaus A, Roland PE, Zilles K. Asymmetry in the human motor cortex and handedness. Neuroimage 1996; 4: 216-222AmuntsKSchlaugGSchleicherASteinmetzHDabringhausARolandPEZillesKAsymmetry in the human motor cortex and handednessNeuroimage1996421622210.1006/nimg.1996.0073Search in Google Scholar

Andersen KW, Siebner HR. Mapping dexterity and handedness: recent insights and future challenges. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 2018; 20: 123-129AndersenKWSiebnerHRMapping dexterity and handedness: recent insights and future challengesCurrent Opinion in Behavioral Sciences20182012312910.1016/j.cobeha.2017.12.020Search in Google Scholar

Asaka T, Wang Y, Fukushima J, Latash ML. Learning effects on muscle modes and multi-mode postural synergies. Exp Brain Res 2008; 184: 323-338AsakaTWangYFukushimaJLatashMLLearning effects on muscle modes and multi-mode postural synergiesExp Brain Res200818432333810.1007/s00221-007-1101-2Search in Google Scholar

Bernstein NA. The co-ordination and regulation of movements. Pergamon Press, Oxford 1967;BernsteinNAThe co-ordination and regulation of movementsPergamon PressOxford1967Search in Google Scholar

Bizzi E, Cheung VC. The neural origin of muscle synergies. Front Comput Neurosci 2013; 7: 51BizziECheungVCThe neural origin of muscle synergiesFront Comput Neurosci201375110.3389/fncom.2013.00051Search in Google Scholar

Bizzi E, Mussa-Ivaldi FA, Giszter S. Computations underlying the execution of movement: a biological perspective. Science 1991; 253: 287-291BizziEMussa-IvaldiFAGiszterSComputations underlying the execution of movement: a biological perspectiveScience199125328729110.1126/science.1857964Search in Google Scholar

Borod JC, Caron HS, Koff E. Left-handers and right-handers compared on performance and preference measures of lateral dominance. Br J Psychol 1984; 75 (Pt 2): 177-186BorodJCCaronHSKoffELeft-handers and right-handers compared on performance and preference measures of lateral dominanceBr J Psychol198475Pt 217718610.1111/j.2044-8295.1984.tb01889.xSearch in Google Scholar

Brouwer B, Sale MV, Nordstrom MA. Asymmetry of motor cortex excitability during a simple motor task: relationships with handedness and manual performance. Exp Brain Res 2001; 138: 467-476BrouwerBSaleMVNordstromMAAsymmetry of motor cortex excitability during a simple motor task: relationships with handedness and manual performanceExp Brain Res200113846747610.1007/s002210100730Search in Google Scholar

Bryden MP. Measuring handedness with questionnaires. Neuropsychologia 1977; 15: 617-624BrydenMPMeasuring handedness with questionnairesNeuropsychologia19771561762410.1016/0028-3932(77)90067-7Search in Google Scholar

Cheung VCK, Cheung BMF, Zhang JH, Chan ZYS, Ha SCW, Chen CY, Cheung RTH. Plasticity of muscle synergies through fractionation and merging during development and training of human runners. Nat Commun 2020; 11: 4356CheungVCKCheungBMFZhangJHChanZYSHaSCWChenCYCheungRTHPlasticity of muscle synergies through fractionation and merging during development and training of human runnersNat Commun202011435610.1038/s41467-020-18210-4745934632868777Search in Google Scholar

Dassonville P, Zhu XH, Uurbil K, Kim SG, Ashe J. Functional activation in motor cortex reflects the direction and the degree of handedness. Proc Natl Acad Sci US A 1997; 94: 14015-14018DassonvillePZhuXHUurbilKKimSGAsheJFunctional activation in motor cortex reflects the direction and the degree of handednessProc Natl Acad Sci US A199794140151401810.1073/pnas.94.25.14015Search in Google Scholar

Duthilleul N, Pirondini E, Coscia M, Micera S. Effect of handedness on muscle synergies during upper limb planar movements. Annu Int Conf IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc 2015; 2015: 3452-3455DuthilleulNPirondiniECosciaMMiceraSEffect of handedness on muscle synergies during upper limb planar movementsAnnu Int Conf IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc201520153452345510.1109/EMBC.2015.7319135Search in Google Scholar

Frere J, Hug F. Between-subject variability of muscle synergies during a complex motor skill. Front Comput Neurosci 2012; 6: 99FrereJHugFBetween-subject variability of muscle synergies during a complex motor skillFront Comput Neurosci201269910.3389/fncom.2012.00099Search in Google Scholar

Gentner R, Gorges S, Weise D, aufm Kampe K, Buttmann M, Classen J. Encoding of motor skill in the corticomuscular system of musicians. Curr Biol 2010; 20: 1869-1874GentnerRGorgesSWeiseDaufmKampe KButtmannMClassenJ.Encoding of motor skill in the corticomuscular system of musiciansCurr Biol2010201869187410.1016/j.cub.2010.09.045Search in Google Scholar

Goble DJ, Brown SH. The biological and behavioral basis of upper limb asymmetries in sensorimotor performance. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2008; 32: 598-610GobleDJBrownSHThe biological and behavioral basis of upper limb asymmetries in sensorimotor performanceNeurosci Biobehav Rev20083259861010.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.10.006Search in Google Scholar

Günay SY, Yarossi M, Brooks DH, Tunik E, Erdogmus D. Transfer learning using low-dimensional subspaces for EMG-based classification of hand posture. Int IEEE EMBS Conf Neural Eng 2019; 2019: 1097-1100GünaySYYarossiMBrooksDHTunikEErdogmusD.Transfer learning using low-dimensional subspaces for EMG-based classification of hand postureInt IEEE EMBS Conf Neural Eng201920191097110010.1109/NER.2019.8717180Search in Google Scholar

Hammond G. Correlates of human handedness in primary motor cortex: a review and hypothesis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2002; 26: 285-292HammondGCorrelates of human handedness in primary motor cortex: a review and hypothesisNeurosci Biobehav Rev20022628529210.1016/S0149-7634(02)00003-9Search in Google Scholar

Hart CB, Giszter SF. A neural basis for motor primitives in the spinal cord. J Neurosci 2010; 30: 1322-1336HartCBGiszterSFA neural basis for motor primitives in the spinal cordJ Neurosci2010301322133610.1523/JNEUROSCI.5894-08.2010663378520107059Search in Google Scholar

Jacobs-Skolik SL, Liang D, Brooks DH, Erdogmus D, Yarossi M, Tunik E. A Muscle Synergy Framework for Cross-Limb Reconstruction of Hand Muscle Activity Distal to a Virtual Wrist-Level Disarticulation. Annu Int Conf IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc 2020; 2020: 3285-3288Jacobs-SkolikSLLiangDBrooksDHErdogmusDYarossiMTunikE.A Muscle Synergy Framework for Cross-Limb Reconstruction of Hand Muscle Activity Distal to a Virtual Wrist-Level DisarticulationAnnu Int Conf IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc202020203285328810.1109/EMBC44109.2020.917593933018706Search in Google Scholar

Kargo WJ, Giszter SF. Individual premotor drive pulses, not time-varying synergies, are the units of adjustment for limb trajectories constructed in spinal cord. J Neurosci 2008; 28: 2409-2425KargoWJGiszterSFIndividual premotor drive pulses, not time-varying synergies, are the units of adjustment for limb trajectories constructed in spinal cordJ Neurosci2008282409242510.1523/JNEUROSCI.3229-07.2008667119418322087Search in Google Scholar

Kutch JJ, Valero-Cuevas FJ. Challenges and new approaches to proving the existence of muscle synergies of neural origin. PLoS Comput Biol 2012; 8: e1002434KutchJJValero-CuevasFJChallenges and new approaches to proving the existence of muscle synergies of neural originPLoS Comput Biol20128e100243410.1371/journal.pcbi.1002434Search in Google Scholar

Latash ML. The bliss (not the problem) of motor abundance (not redundancy). Exp Brain Res 2012; 217: 1-5LatashMLThe bliss (not the problem) of motor abundance (not redundancy)Exp Brain Res20122171510.1007/s00221-012-3000-4Search in Google Scholar

Lawrence DG, Kuypers HG. The functional organization of the motor system in the monkey. I. The effects of bilateral pyramidal lesions. Brain 1968; 91: 1-14LawrenceDGKuypersHGThe functional organization of the motor system in the monkeyI. The effects of bilateral pyramidal lesions. Brain196891114Search in Google Scholar

Lee DD, Seung HS. Learning the parts of objects by non-negative matrix factorization. Nature 1999; 401: 788791LeeDDSeungHSLearning the parts of objects by non-negative matrix factorizationNature199940178879110.1038/44565Search in Google Scholar

Leo A, Handjaras G, Bianchi M, Marino H, Gabiccini M, Guidi A, Scilingo EP, Pietrini P, Bicchi A, Santello M. A synergy-based hand control is encoded in human motor cortical areas. Elife 2016; 5: e13420LeoAHandjarasGBianchiMMarinoHGabicciniMGuidiAScilingoEPPietriniPBicchiASantelloMA synergy-based hand control is encoded in human motor cortical areasElife20165e1342010.7554/eLife.13420Search in Google Scholar

Mani S, Przybyla A, Good DC, Haaland KY, Sainburg RL. Contralesional Arm Preference Depends on Hemisphere of Damage and Target Location in Unilateral Stroke Patients. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 2014; 28: 584-593ManiSPrzybylaAGoodDCHaalandKYSainburgRLContralesional Arm Preference Depends on Hemisphere of Damage and Target Location in Unilateral Stroke PatientsNeurorehabil Neural Repair20142858459310.1177/1545968314520720Search in Google Scholar

Muir RB, Lemon RN. Corticospinal neurons with a special role in precision grip. Brain Res 1983; 261: 312-316MuirRBLemonRNCorticospinal neurons with a special role in precision gripBrain Res198326131231610.1016/0006-8993(83)90635-2Search in Google Scholar

Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia 1971; 9: 97-113OldfieldRCThe assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventoryNeuropsychologia197199711310.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4Search in Google Scholar

Pellegrino L, Coscia M, Casadio M. Muscle activities in similar arms performing identical tasks reveal the neural basis of muscle synergies. Exp Brain Res 2020; 238: 121-138PellegrinoLCosciaMCasadioMMuscle activities in similar arms performing identical tasks reveal the neural basis of muscle synergiesExp Brain Res202023812113810.1007/s00221-019-05679-931807808Search in Google Scholar

Pool EM, Rehme AK, Fink GR, Eickhoff SB, Grefkes C. Handedness and effective connectivity of the motor system. Neuroimage 2014; 99: 451-460PoolEMRehmeAKFinkGREickhoffSBGrefkesCHandedness and effective connectivity of the motor systemNeuroimage20149945146010.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.048498254724862079Search in Google Scholar

Przybyla A, Good DC, Sainburg RL. Dynamic dominance varies with handedness: reduced interlimb asymmetries in left-handers. Exp Brain Res 2012; 216: 419-431PrzybylaAGoodDCSainburgRLDynamic dominance varies with handedness: reduced interlimb asymmetries in left-handersExp Brain Res201221641943110.1007/s00221-011-2946-y370926222113487Search in Google Scholar

Rathelot JA, Strick PL. Muscle representation in the macaque motor cortex: an anatomical perspective. Proc Natl Acad Sci US A 2006; 103: 8257-8262RathelotJAStrickPLMuscle representation in the macaque motor cortex: an anatomical perspectiveProc Natl Acad Sci US A20061038257826210.1073/pnas.0602933103146140716702556Search in Google Scholar

Rathelot JA, Strick PL. Subdivisions of primary motor cortex based on cortico-motoneuronal cells. Proc Natl Acad Sci US A 2009; 106: 918-923RathelotJAStrickPLSubdivisions of primary motor cortex based on cortico-motoneuronal cellsProc Natl Acad Sci US A200910691892310.1073/pnas.0808362106262125019139417Search in Google Scholar

Roh J, Rymer WZ, Perreault EJ, Yoo SB, Beer RF. Alterations in upper limb muscle synergy structure in chronic stroke survivors. J Neurophysiol 2013; 109: 768-781RohJRymerWZPerreaultEJYooSBBeerRFAlterations in upper limb muscle synergy structure in chronic stroke survivorsJ Neurophysiol201310976878110.1152/jn.00670.2012356738923155178Search in Google Scholar

Safavynia SA, Ting LH. Task-level feedback can explain temporal recruitment of spatially fixed muscle synergies throughout postural perturbations. J Neurophysiol 2012; 107: 159-177SafavyniaSATingLHTask-level feedback can explain temporal recruitment of spatially fixed muscle synergies throughout postural perturbationsJ Neurophysiol201210715917710.1152/jn.00653.2011334968821957219Search in Google Scholar

Sainburg RL. Evidence for a dynamic-dominance hypothesis of handedness. Exp Brain Res 2002; 142: 241-258SainburgRLEvidence for a dynamic-dominance hypothesis of handednessExp Brain Res200214224125810.1007/s00221-001-0913-811807578Search in Google Scholar

Sainburg RL. Handedness: differential specializations for control of trajectory and position. Exerc Sport Sci Rev 2005; 33: 206-213SainburgRLHandedness: differential specializations for control of trajectory and positionExerc Sport Sci Rev20053320621310.1097/00003677-200510000-0001016239839Search in Google Scholar

Sainburg RL. Convergent models of handedness and brain lateralization. Front Psychol 2014; 5: 1092SainburgRLConvergent models of handedness and brain lateralizationFront Psychol20145109210.3389/fpsyg.2014.01092418933225339923Search in Google Scholar

Sawers A, Allen JL, Ting LH. Long-term training modifies the modular structure and organization of walking balance control. J Neurophysiol 2015; 114: 3359-3373SawersAAllenJLTingLHLong-term training modifies the modular structure and organization of walking balance controlJ Neurophysiol20151143359337310.1152/jn.00758.2015486837926467521Search in Google Scholar

Schaefer SY, Haaland KY, Sainburg RL. Dissociation of initial trajectory and final position errors during visuomotor adaptation following unilateral stroke. Brain Res 2009; 1298: 78-91SchaeferSYHaalandKYSainburgRLDissociation of initial trajectory and final position errors during visuomotor adaptation following unilateral strokeBrain Res20091298789110.1016/j.brainres.2009.08.063315149219728993Search in Google Scholar

Schaefer SY, Mutha PK, Haaland KY, Sainburg RL. Hemispheric specialization for movement control produces dissociable differences in online corrections after stroke. Cereb Cortex 2012; 22: 1407-1419SchaeferSYMuthaPKHaalandKYSainburgRLHemispheric specialization for movement control produces dissociable differences in online corrections after strokeCereb Cortex2012221407141910.1093/cercor/bhr237335718021878488Search in Google Scholar

Solodkin A, Hlustik P, Noll DC, Small SL. Lateralization of motor circuits and handedness during finger movements. Eur J Neurol 2001; 8: 425-434SolodkinAHlustikPNollDCSmallSLLateralization of motor circuits and handedness during finger movementsEur J Neurol2001842543410.1046/j.1468-1331.2001.00242.xSearch in Google Scholar

Tresch MC, Cheung VC, d'Avella A. Matrix factorization algorithms for the identification of muscle synergies: evaluation on simulated and experimental data sets. J Neurophysiol 2006; 95: 2199-2212TreschMCCheungVCd'AvellaA.Matrix factorization algorithms for the identification of muscle synergies: evaluation on simulated and experimental data setsJ Neurophysiol2006952199221210.1152/jn.00222.2005Search in Google Scholar

Tresch MC, Jarc A. The case for and against muscle synergies. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2009; 19: 601-607TreschMCJarcAThe case for and against muscle synergiesCurr Opin Neurobiol20091960160710.1016/j.conb.2009.09.002Search in Google Scholar

Tresch MC, Saltiel P, Bizzi E. The construction of movement by the spinal cord. Nat Neurosci 1999; 2: 162-167TreschMCSaltielPBizziEThe construction of movement by the spinal cordNat Neurosci1999216216710.1038/5721Search in Google Scholar

Triggs WJ, Subramanium B, Rossi F. Hand preference and transcranial magnetic stimulation asymmetry of cortical motor representation. Brain Res 1999; 835: 324-329TriggsWJSubramaniumBRossiFHand preference and transcranial magnetic stimulation asymmetry of cortical motor representationBrain Res199983532432910.1016/S0006-8993(99)01629-7Search in Google Scholar

Volkmann J, Schnitzler A, Witte OW, Freund H. Handedness and asymmetry of hand representation in human motor cortex. J Neurophysiol 1998; 79: 2149-2154VolkmannJSchnitzlerAWitteOWFreundHHandedness and asymmetry of hand representation in human motor cortexJ Neurophysiol1998792149215410.1152/jn.1998.79.4.2149Search in Google Scholar

Woytowicz EJ, Westlake KP, Whitall J, Sainburg RL. Handedness results from complementary hemispheric dominance, not global hemispheric dominance: evidence from mechanically coupled bilateral movements. J Neurophysiol 2018; 120: 729-740WoytowiczEJWestlakeKPWhitallJSainburgRLHandedness results from complementary hemispheric dominance, not global hemispheric dominance: evidence from mechanically coupled bilateral movementsJ Neurophysiol201812072974010.1152/jn.00878.2017Search in Google Scholar

Zaaimi B, Dean LR, Baker SN. Different contributions of primary motor cortex, reticular formation, and spinal cord to fractionated muscle activation. J Neurophysiol 2018; 119: 235-250ZaaimiBDeanLRBakerSNDifferent contributions of primary motor cortex, reticular formation, and spinal cord to fractionated muscle activationJ Neurophysiol201811923525010.1152/jn.00672.2017Search in Google Scholar

Zhang W, Sainburg RL, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML. Hand dominance and multi-finger synergies. Neurosci Lett 2006; 409: 200-204ZhangWSainburgRLZatsiorskyVMLatashMLHand dominance and multi-finger synergiesNeurosci Lett200640920020410.1016/j.neulet.2006.09.048Search in Google Scholar

Ziemann U, Hallett M. Hemispheric asymmetry of ipsilateral motor cortex activation during unimanual motor tasks: further evidence for motor dominance. Clinical Neurophysiology 2001; 112: 107-113ZiemannUHallettMHemispheric asymmetry of ipsilateral motor cortex activation during unimanual motor tasks: further evidence for motor dominanceClinical Neurophysiology200111210711310.1016/S1388-2457(00)00502-2Search in Google Scholar

Articoli consigliati da Trend MD

Pianifica la tua conferenza remota con Sciendo