1. bookVolumen 55 (2017): Edición 1 (January 2017)
Detalles de la revista
License
Formato
Revista
eISSN
1899-7562
Primera edición
13 Jan 2009
Calendario de la edición
5 veces al año
Idiomas
Inglés
Acceso abierto

The Relationship Between Stress and Coping in Table Tennis

Publicado en línea: 30 Jan 2017
Volumen & Edición: Volumen 55 (2017) - Edición 1 (January 2017)
Páginas: 75 - 81
Detalles de la revista
License
Formato
Revista
eISSN
1899-7562
Primera edición
13 Jan 2009
Calendario de la edición
5 veces al año
Idiomas
Inglés
Introduction

Competitive anxiety and stress management are part of any kind of competitions. Competitive anxiety is a mental state which includes cognitive, somatic and emotional components (Gillham et al., 2014; Jones, 1995; Mellalieu et al., 2009; Woodman and Hardy, 2001). All these components affect how athletes handle stress (Mellalieu et al., 2009; Patel et al., 2010).

There are many different factors such as age, gender, life experience, skill level (Gan et al., 2009; Radochonski et al., 2011), control of the situation (Jones, 1995) and self-confidence (Gillham et al., 2014; Jones, 1995; Mellalieu et al., 2009; Wagstaff et al., 2011), which have an impact on competitive anxiety. Using different coping strategies can also influence the effects of stress symptoms (for example worry and heart rate). Stress can even be interpreted as facilitative and can play an important role in performing well in competitive situations (Gillham et al., 2014; Mellalieu et al., 2009; Neil et al., 2006).

Sports-related coping research has found that problem-focused strategies are associated with positive effects, while emotion-focused strategies and avoidance coping strategies are associated with negative effects (Crocker and Graham, 1995; Kashdan et al., 2006). However, Krohne and Hindel (1988) found that elite German table tennis players who used cognitive avoidance coping strategies during a match were more successful (they won more matches) compared to players who did not employ these strategies. In addition, avoidance coping was associated with less state anxiety after making performance errors compared to the use of problem-focused coping (Krohne and Hindel, 1988). Problem-focused coping strategies are targeted toward stressful situations and resolution of the problem, while avoidance coping protects against interfering thoughts and actions. Nevertheless, Roth and Cohen (1986) found that avoidance coping was better than problem-focused coping if the situation was uncontrollable, whereas problem-focused coping was better when there was the potential for control. Hence, in the Krohne and Hindel’s study (1988), elite German table tennis players’ avoidance coping was associated with reduced state anxiety and better performance as the situation was uncontrollable and there was a low level of situational control. Supporting this fact, Kaisidis-Rodafinos et al. (1997) suggested that under certain circumstances such as low situational control, avoidance coping might be more effective in reducing stress than problem-focused coping. Another study by Anshel and Si (2008) also supported the idea that copying style was a function of the type of a stressful event, where “stressful event” was defined as when an athlete felt worry or anxiety about an upcoming event, thus their focus was only on cognitive symptoms. Moreover, Anshel and Si (2008) found that avoidance coping could be more common than problem-focused coping in certain stressful events in elite Chinese athletes.

Hatzigeorgiadis and Chroni (2007) studied Greek elite male swimmers’ precompetitive anxiety and coping strategies during competition. Specifically, the participants of the study completed a short test of anxiety intensity and direction before their competition and a coping questionnaire after their event. According to the findings, the intensity of cognitive anxiety had low to moderate correlation with a coping approach (problem-focused strategies, however, only planning was significant) and a low to moderate positive relationship with avoidance coping strategies (none of which were statistically significant).

In another study, Ntoumanes and Biddle (2000) examined how coping strategies related to the level of intensity and direction (debilitative or facilitative) of anxiety of sports. British university athletes had to recall a recent stressful situation in their sport, the coping strategies that they used and the intensity and direction of their anxiety symptoms. The perception of facilitative cognitive anxiety (facilitative direction of anxiety) was related to problem-focused coping, while high levels of cognitive intensity were related to emotion focused coping and avoidance.

Based on the Ntoumanis and Biddle’s study (2000), our assumption was that table tennis players with high cognitive competitive anxiety intensity would report greater use of emotion-focused and avoidance coping strategies compared to those with low cognitive competitive anxiety intensity. Furthermore, gender differences were expected between the level of anxiety and the preferred coping style.

Material and Methods
Participants

One hundred and two (102) competitive table tennis players aged 10 to 60 (80 males and 20 females) from the United States participated in the study. The participants’ table tennis level was mainly intermediate based on the American ranking system. Intermediate players’ rankings were between 1000 and 1700 and advanced players were between 1700 and 2200.

Measures

The relationship of competitive anxiety and coping strategies in sport was studied based on a quantitative method of research using the SPSS (Statistical Package for the Social Sciences) program for statistical analysis. The study examined how independent variables (competitive cognitive anxiety intensity factors) influenced dependent variables (coping strategies). Furthermore, gender differences in the different use of coping strategies as well as the intensity and direction of competitive anxiety were investigated.

Procedures

The athletes were verbally invited to participate at table tennis tournaments in the USA and were asked to complete a demographic information sheet and the Revised Competitive State Anxiety Inventory-2 (CSAI-2-R; Cox et al., 2003) a minimum of 30 and maximum of 90 min before the start of the competitive event. The timing of the inventory administration might have had an effect on the athletes’ game, although the researchers did not receive any negative feedback, comments or complaints from the participants. Additionally, participants were asked to complete the Modified Cope questionnaire (MCOPE; Crocker and Graham, 1995) 15 min after their match.

The participants were protected following the ethical standards taken from the APA Code of Conducts (2003). Informed consent documents emphasizing confidentiality were given to each participant and discussed in detail prior to the questionnaires. Assent information was included to participants that were minors. Furthermore, a consent form was given to participants who were over age 18 and to the minors’ parents/guardians. The researchers verified that the participants understood the documents and the research procedures.

Statistical Analysis

One-way ANOVA was used to examine how independent variables (competitive cognitive anxiety intensity factors) influenced dependent variables (coping strategies). Competitive anxiety intensity factors (high/low intensity) were the dependent variables and the 12 coping strategies were dependent variables. The 12 coping strategies were active coping, seeking social support for instrumental reasons, planning, increasing effort, suppression of competitive activities (these are problem-focused strategies), seeking social support for emotional reasons, denial, humor, venting of emotion, self-blame, wishful thinking (they are known as emotion-focused strategies) and behavioural disengagement (it is classified as avoidance coping).

Using the median of the competitive cognitive anxiety intensity scores, two grouping variables created for each ANOVA (high/low intensity). Participants who had exactly the same score as the median were taken out from the sample (i.e. 13 athletes). The cognitive anxiety intensity median was 20.00. Therefore, those who had lower intensity scores than the median were put into a low intensity category (N = 48), while those who had higher scores than the median were placed into a high intensity category (N= 54).

A t-test was used to examine gender differences in the different use of coping strategies as well as the intensity and direction of competitive anxiety.

Results

The high cognitive anxiety intensity group used significantly more (p ≤ 0.05) avoidance coping (behavioural disengagement) compared to the low cognitive anxiety intensity group. Moreover, athletes in the high cognitive competitive anxiety intensity group used significantly more (p ≤ 0.01) denial coping strategies (emotion focused coping) compared to athletes who were in the low cognitive anxiety group (Table 1 and Figure 1).

Significant differences between high and low cognitive anxiety groups in the frequency of the use of emotion focused coping and avoidance

High Cognitive Anxiety IntensityLow Cognitive Anxiety Intensity
MeanStd ErrorMeanStd Errordfdf errorFp
Seeking social support12.594
for instrumental reasons0.81910.5400.6281963.9590.049

p≤ 0.01

Seeking social support11.789
for emotional reasons0.80510.1270.6171962.6860.105
Behavior disengagement7.8070.5346.0900.4101966.5040.012

p≤ 0.01

Self-blame11.7450.71411.8790.5471960.0220.881
Planning13.7480.68314.7970.5241961.4840.226
Suppression of competitive activities11.0830.70512.0940.5401961.2950.258
Venting of emotions9.8090.7237.9100.5551964.3420.040

p≤ 0.01

Humor9.3720.7998.8800.6121960.2390.626
Increasing Effort15.3410.61416.3250.4711961.6180.206
Wishful thinking11.9880.73710.8860.5651961.4080.238
Active coping14.3040.66114.7900.5061960.3410.561
Denial9.6740.5527.6580.4231968.3890.005

p≤ 0.01

Significant differences between high and low cognitive anxiety groups in the frequency of the use of emotion focused coping and avoidance

Results from the study showed that gender differences did exist in cognitive anxiety intensity and direction. Women rated their cognitive anxiety higher than their male counterparts (Table 2). However, researchers could not verify any significant gender differences in copying style.

Significant gender differences in cognitive anxiety intensity and direction

GenderNMeanSDtp
Cognitive AnxietyMale8020.256.08-2.070.041

p≤0.01

FemaleIntensity2023.729.54
Cognitive AnxietyMale78-1.7110.522.340.021

p≤ 0.05

DirectionFemale22-8.2214.49

Discussion

The present study examined the relationships between cognitive competitive anxiety intensity and coping strategies in table tennis players. Our study found significant difference between low and high cognitive competitive anxiety groups with regard to the use of coping strategies.

A significantly higher use of avoidance coping in the high cognitive anxiety group compared to low cognitive competitive anxiety group is congruent with the results of the Hatzigeorgiadis and Chroni’s study (2007). They also found low to moderate positive correlations between avoidance coping and the intensity of cognitive anxiety in competitive male swimmers even if their result was not statistically significant. Furthermore, Hatzigeorgiadis and Chroni (2007) examined only approach coping (problem-focused coping) along with avoidance coping and their study did not include emotion-focused coping strategies. Additionally, only male swimmers were surveyed in their research and their sample size was limited. The use of the Mental Readiness Form to assess pre-competition anxiety (MRF-3, Thomas et al., 1999) considering the validity of a single instrument is also questionable. Hence, although our result is congruent with the one reported in the Hatzigeorgiadis and Chroni’s study (2007), our study highlights the possibility of some relationship between avoidance coping and a high level of cognitive anxiety even if we cannot make further generalizations based on this result.

In certain conditions, avoidance coping can be effective in the short term (Kim and Duda, 2003) and might be a more effective coping strategy than approach coping (problem-focused coping) (Kaisidis-Rodafinos et al., 1997). Yet, in a long-term perspective, problem-focused coping was positively related to, whereas avoidance was negatively related to, satisfaction, enjoyment and desire to continue (Kim and Duda, 2003). This might imply that avoidance coping can be effective during table tennis competition, but not in the long term. When referring to the Krohne and Hindel’s study (1988), it is important to note that they did not measure the perceived control of athletes’ emotions during the competitive event. When athletes interpret their anxiety as facilitative, they might assume feeling greater control, while interpreting their anxiety as debilitative might assume they feel less control. This study’s results also contradict the Roth and Cohen’s study (1986) which suggested different use of coping strategies as a function of perceived control. According to them, approach coping (problem-focused coping) is preferable when stressful situations are perceived as highly controllable, while avoidance coping is more frequently used when there is low perceived control. However, in our study we did not find a significant difference in the effectiveness of these factors.

One of the key factors in determining effective coping strategies is how athletes interpret their anxiety (facilitative or debilitative) which in turn can help them cope with stress more effectively and assist in the choice of most appropriate coping strategies (problem-focused, emotion-focused or avoidance coping) in the competition environment.

It is essential to point out that Ntoumous and Biddle’s (2000) and Hatzigeorgiadis et al.’s (2007) study participants were British and Greek, respectively. Thus, our results (obtained in US residents) could be different by virtue of cultural differences. Based on previous studies, cultural differences could influence coping strategies and perceived controllability (Puente-Diaz and Anshel, 2005).

Another finding of the present study concerned gender differences that did exist in cognitive anxiety intensity and direction. Women rated their cognitive anxiety higher and more negatively compared to male athletes; however, there were significant differences between the number of men (N = 80) and women (N = 22) in the sample, which could distort the results.

This study has several limitations. First, because of the nature of the sample, we can only apply the results to table tennis. Nevertheless, this can also be an opportunity for other sports to compare their results with table tennis. Second, our research did not focus on a particular age group or level per se, therefore, the group of participants is not homogenous in regard to age and sports level of the table tennis players in this study’s sample. This could limit some elements for generalization and practical implications. However, it is important to highlight that in the American table tennis tournament system, the age range and sporting level of the participants are typically very wide. Thus, it was very hard to control our sample’s homogeneity and ensure a sufficient number of participants in the study.

Future research should repeat this study with a more homogenous sample. Thirdly, the research did not include the factor of winning and losing. It did not take into consideration whether the athletes won or lost when they completed the post-match Modified Cope questionnaire (MCOPE; Crocker and Graham, 1995) to define what coping strategies were most successful in a competitive environment. Furthermore, the findings do not allow us to draw correlational or causal relationships between anxiety intensity, direction and coping strategies.

The results suggest that there is some connection between cognitive competitive anxiety intensity and coping strategies. They indicate that cognitive competitive anxiety intensity might predict what kinds of coping strategies athletes would use during competition.

Based on our findings, coaches and sport psychologists should assess their athletes’ level of cognitive anxiety to help them develop effective coping strategies and appropriate stress management techniques. Furthermore, gender differences in competitive anxiety should be considered as well. Confidence and arousal management strategies such as positive self-talk imagery and deep breathing should be taught to athletes to assist them to cope with their competitive anxiety successfully. Additionally, our study also points out that anxiety responses are normal in stressful situations such as competitions and can even be facilitative.

Significant differences between high and low cognitive anxiety groups in the frequency of the use of emotion focused coping and avoidance
Significant differences between high and low cognitive anxiety groups in the frequency of the use of emotion focused coping and avoidance

Significant differences between high and low cognitive anxiety groups in the frequency of the use of emotion focused coping and avoidance

High Cognitive Anxiety IntensityLow Cognitive Anxiety Intensity
MeanStd ErrorMeanStd Errordfdf errorFp
Seeking social support12.594
for instrumental reasons0.81910.5400.6281963.9590.049

p≤ 0.01

Seeking social support11.789
for emotional reasons0.80510.1270.6171962.6860.105
Behavior disengagement7.8070.5346.0900.4101966.5040.012

p≤ 0.01

Self-blame11.7450.71411.8790.5471960.0220.881
Planning13.7480.68314.7970.5241961.4840.226
Suppression of competitive activities11.0830.70512.0940.5401961.2950.258
Venting of emotions9.8090.7237.9100.5551964.3420.040

p≤ 0.01

Humor9.3720.7998.8800.6121960.2390.626
Increasing Effort15.3410.61416.3250.4711961.6180.206
Wishful thinking11.9880.73710.8860.5651961.4080.238
Active coping14.3040.66114.7900.5061960.3410.561
Denial9.6740.5527.6580.4231968.3890.005

p≤ 0.01

Significant gender differences in cognitive anxiety intensity and direction

GenderNMeanSDtp
Cognitive AnxietyMale8020.256.08-2.070.041

p≤0.01

FemaleIntensity2023.729.54
Cognitive AnxietyMale78-1.7110.522.340.021

p≤ 0.05

DirectionFemale22-8.2214.49

Ansel MH, Si G. Copying styles following acute stress in sport among elite Chinese athletes: a test of trait and transactional copying theories. J Sport Behav, 2008; 31: 3-21AnselMH,SiG.Copying styles following acute stress in sport among elite Chinese athletes: a test of trait and transactional copying theoriesJ Sport Behav200831321Search in Google Scholar

Cox RH, Martens MP, Russell WD. Measuring anxiety in athletics: The revised Competitive State Anxiety Inventory-2. J Sport and Exercise Psy, 2003; 25: 519-533CoxRH,MartensMP,RussellWD.Measuring anxiety in athletics: The revised Competitive State Anxiety Inventory-2J Sport and Exercise Psy20032551953310.1123/jsep.25.4.519Search in Google Scholar

Crocker PRE, Graham TR. Coping by competitive athletes with performance stress: Gender differences and relationships with affect. Sport Psychol, 1995; 9: 325-338CrockerPRE,GrahamTR.Coping by competitive athletes with performance stress: Gender differences and relationships with affectSport Psychol1995932533810.1123/tsp.9.3.325Search in Google Scholar

Gan Q, Anshel MH, Kim JK. Sources and cognitive appraisals of acute stress as predictors of coping style among male and female Chinese athletes. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 2009; 7: 68-88GanQ,AnshelMH,KimJK.Sources and cognitive appraisals of acute stress as predictors of coping style among male and female Chinese athletesInternational Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology20097688810.1080/1612197X.2009.9671893Search in Google Scholar

Gillham E, Gillham AD. Identifying athletes’ sources of competitive state anxiety. Journal of Sport Behavior, 2014 37: 1-16GillhamE,GillhamAD.Identifying athletes’ sources of competitive state anxietyJournal of Sport Behavior201437116Search in Google Scholar

Hatzigeorgiadis A, Chroni S. Pre-competition anxiety and in-competition coping experienced male swimmers. Int J Sports Sci Coach, 2007; 2: 181-189HatzigeorgiadisA,ChroniS.Pre-competition anxiety and in-competition coping experienced male swimmersInt J Sports Sci Coach2007218118910.1260/174795407781394310Search in Google Scholar

Jones G. More than just a game: Research developments and issues in competitive anxiety in sport. British Journal of Psychology, 1995; 86: 449-478JonesG.More than just a game: Research developments and issues in competitive anxiety in sportBritish Journal of Psychology19958644947810.1111/j.2044-8295.1995.tb02565.x8542197Search in Google Scholar

Kaisidis-Rodafinos A, Anshel MA, Porter A. Personal and situational factors that predict coping strategies for acute stress among basketball referees. J Sport Sci, 1997; 15: 427-436Kaisidis-RodafinosA,AnshelMA,PorterA.Personal and situational factors that predict coping strategies for acute stress among basketball refereesJ Sport Sci19971542743610.1080/0264041973672189293419Search in Google Scholar

Kashdan TB, Barrios V, Forsyth JP, Steger MF. Experiential avoidance as a generalized psychological vulnerability: Comparisons with coping and emotion regulation strategies. Behav Res Ther, 2006; 44: 1301–1320KashdanTB,BarriosV,ForsythJP,StegerMF.Experiential avoidance as a generalized psychological vulnerability: Comparisons with coping and emotion regulation strategiesBehav Res Ther2006441301132010.1016/j.brat.2005.10.00316321362Search in Google Scholar

Kim MS, Duda JL. The Coping Process: Cognitive appraisal of stress, coping strategies, and coping effectiveness. Sport Psychol, 2003; 17: 406-423KimMS,DudaJL.The Coping Process: Cognitive appraisal of stress, coping strategies, and coping effectivenessSport Psychol20031740642310.1123/tsp.17.4.406Search in Google Scholar

Krohne HW, Hindel C. Trait anxiety, state anxiety, and coping behavior as predictors of athletic performance. Anxiety Res, 1988; 1: 225-234KrohneHW,HindelC.Trait anxiety, state anxiety, and coping behavior as predictors of athletic performanceAnxiety Res1988122523410.1080/08917778808248721Search in Google Scholar

Mellalieu SD, Hanton S, Fletcher D. A competitive anxiety review: recent directions in sport psychology research. Literature reviews in sport psychology. S. Hanton and SD. Mellalieu (Eds), New York: Nova Science Publishers, 1-45; 2009MellalieuSD,HantonS,FletcherD.A competitive anxiety review: recent directions in sport psychology researchLiterature reviews in sport psychologyHantonS.MellalieuSD.New YorkNova Science Publishers145200910.1080/17509840701827445Search in Google Scholar

Neil R, Mellalieu SD, Hanton S. Psychological skills usage and competitive anxiety response as a function of skill level in rugby union. J Sports Sci Med, 2006; 5: 415-423NeilR,MellalieuSD,HantonS.Psychological skills usage and competitive anxiety response as a function of skill level in rugby unionJ Sports Sci Med20065415423Search in Google Scholar

Ntoumanis N, Biddle SJH. Relationship of intensity and direction of competitive anxiety with coping strategies. The Sport Psychologist, 2000; 14: 360-371NtoumanisN,BiddleSJH.Relationship of intensity and direction of competitive anxiety with coping strategiesThe Sport Psychologist20001436037110.1123/tsp.14.4.360Search in Google Scholar

Patel DR, Omar H, Terry M. Sport-related performance anxiety in young female athletes. J Pediatr Adolesc Gynecol, 2010; 23: 325-335PatelDR,OmarH,TerryM.Sport-related performance anxiety in young female athletesJ Pediatr Adolesc Gynecol20102332533510.1016/j.jpag.2010.04.00420869282Search in Google Scholar

Puente-Diaz R, Anshel MH. Sources of acute stress, cognitive appraisal, and coping strategies among highly skilled mexican and U.S. competitive tennis players. J Soc Psychol, 2005; 4: 429-446Puente-DiazR,AnshelMH.Sources of acute stress, cognitive appraisal, and coping strategies among highly skilled mexican and U.S. competitive tennis playersJ Soc Psychol2005442944610.3200/SOCP.145.4.429-44616050340Search in Google Scholar

Radochonski M, Cynarski WJ, Perenc L, Siorek-Maslanka L. Competitive anxiety and coping strategies in youth martial arts and track and field athletes. J Hum Kinet, 2011; 27: 181-190RadochonskiM,CynarskiWJ,PerencL,Siorek-MaslankaL.Competitive anxiety and coping strategies in youth martial arts and track and field athletesJ Hum Kinet20112718119010.2478/v10078-011-0014-0Search in Google Scholar

Roth S, Cohen LJ. Approach avoidance, and coping with stress. American Psychologist, 1986; 41: 813-819RothS,CohenLJ.Approach avoidance, and coping with stressAmerican Psychologist19864181381910.1037/0003-066X.41.7.813Search in Google Scholar

Thomas PR, Murphey SM, Hardy L. Test of performance strategies: Development and preliminary validation of a comprehensive measure of athletes’ psychological skills. Journal of Sports Sciences, 1999; 17: 697-711ThomasPR,MurpheySM,HardyL.Test of performance strategies: Development and preliminary validation of a comprehensive measure of athletes’ psychological skillsJournal of Sports Sciences19991769771110.1080/02640419936556010521001Search in Google Scholar

Wagtsaff CRD, Neil R, Mellalieu SD, Hanton S. Key movements in directional research in competitive anxiety. In Neil R, Mellalieu SD, Hanton S (Eds.) Coping and emotion in sport. London: Taylor & Francis, 143-166; 2011WagtsaffCRD,NeilR,MellalieuSD,HantonS.Key movements in directional research in competitive anxietyInNeilR,MellalieuSD,HantonSCoping and emotion in sportLondonTaylor & Francis143166201110.4324/9780203852293_chapter_7Search in Google Scholar

Woodman T, Hardy L. Stress and Anxiety. In Singer R, Hausenblas HA, Janelle CM. (Eds.). Handbook of research on sport psychology, New York: Wiley, 290-318; 2001WoodmanT,HardyL.Stress and AnxietyInSingerR,HausenblasHA,JanelleCM.Handbook of research on sport psychologyNew YorkWiley2903182001Search in Google Scholar

Artículos recomendados de Trend MD

Planifique su conferencia remota con Sciendo