A rugby union game lasts for 80 min and is cognitively and physically demanding. Throughout the game, effective decision making and execution of game-specific strategies are required (Fullagar et al., 2015). Previously, most rugby (union and league) studies have examined how to optimise player performance by focusing on improving position-dependent physical attributes, such as mass and body fat, endocrine levels and power output (Duthie et al., 2003; Kilduff et al., 2015; Shearer et al., 2015). However, sleep is vital in the recovery and performanceof athletes (Gupta et al., 2016). Studies in other team sports such as rugby league (Thornton et al., 2017) and soccer (Fullagar et al., 2016) highlight this importance, yet little is known about sleep behaviours of elite rugby players (Dunican and Eastwood, 2016). To date, only two studies have reported the effects of training and game time on sleep in rugby union players; both utilised wrist-activity monitors to assess sleep on the nights before, the night of and nights after a game, reporting increases in player sleep duration in the nights prior to a game, and delayed sleep onset time and decreased sleep duration on the night of the game (Eagles et al., 2014; Shearer et al., 2015).
The present study used wrist-activity monitors to assess sleep in elite rugby players as they minimally interfere with normal training and sleep in athletes (Lee, 2012). This study also estimated alertness during training and game time via a three process bio-mathematical model (Borbely, 1982), the Sleep, Activity, Fatigue and Task Effectiveness (SAFTE) model (Hursh, 2004). This approach is common in military, aviation and railroad operations in the design of work and rest patterns to maximise alertness and to minimise risk (Dean et al., 2007; Hilaire and Klerman, 2007). To date, no studies have investigated the use of such bio-mathematical modelling in athletic populations to assess alertness.
Typically, during the week of a Super Rugby game, a team of 23 players (15 to start the game, plus 8 substitutes) are selected from a squad of 36 players three days prior to a game. The remaining players (13) continue to train with the team up to the day before the game. At present, very little is known about sleep patterns and their relationship to player alertness in the days leading into a competitive game. Such information could be beneficial to coaches and researchers in understanding pre-game, and post-game sleep behaviours that may be used to support the scheduling of training times to optimise recovery and alertness.
These circumstances provide a unique opportunity to investigate the sleep habits of those selected (and not selected) to play the game and thereby understand more about the sleep behaviours of elite rugby players before and after training and game periods. Therefore, the aims of this study were to: (i) quantify the differences in sleep behaviours in players who played the game and those from the greater squad who did not play during the week of a Super Rugby home game; and (ii) estimate alertness for players in both groups for training and game time.
Thirty-six elite contracted male rugby union players from a Super Rugby team based in Perth, Western Australia participated in this study. Players age was 26 ± 3 years (21-34 years) and body mass 102 ± 11 kg (80-122 kg). Demographic information, health status and sleep history were collected from players via a paper-based survey. Ethics approval was obtained (RA/4/1/7235) and informed consent received from all athletes prior to participation. This study was conducted in compliance with the Declaration of Helsinki for human experimentation.
Measures of sleep were obtained from a wrist-activity monitor, the Readiband™ (v3, analysed using Readiband Sync™) (Fatigue Science Inc., Canada). The wrist-activity monitor was issued to each player at 17:00 on the Wednesday; three days before the game and collected at 08:00 on the Wednesday; four days after the game. These were continuously worn on the non-dominant wrist throughout the 7-day period. The Readiband has good validity (overall accuracy of 93%) when compared to sleep/wake epochs against polysomnography (Russell, 2006), has undergone in-field validation in Australian Rules Football (Dennis et al., 2016) and has been approved by the US Federal Drug Administration (FDA) (Readiband FDA Approval, 2011) as a device for measurement of physical activity and sleep data.
The monitors were downloaded and analysed using the automated Readiband Sync software and its proprietary algorithm. The proprietary algorithm has been shown to compare favourably both to in-laboratory polysomnography (PSG) and another widely used and validated wrist-activity monitor, the ActiGraph device (Dunican et al., 2017). Sleep measures derived included: time at sleep onset (the time the person initiated sleep), sleep latency (time between lights out and sleep onset), sleep duration (time between sleep onset and wake, minus any time awake during this period), wake after sleep onset (WASO) (time spent awake after sleep onset and before final waking time), time at wake (time of final waking, not followed by any additional sleep) and sleep efficiency (percentage of time spent asleep whilst in bed: sleep duration/time in bed minus sleep latency and WASO).
Measures of alertness were calculated using the SAFTE algorithm. SAFTE incorporates a homeostatic sleep reservoir, circadian oscillator and sleep inertia function (Roma, 2012) to generate a measure of alertness (Hursh, 2004). The SAFTE algorithm interface allows the input of variables such as geographical location (longitude and latitude) for the calculation of natural light and dark cycles of that specific location, as these will affect the circadian rhythm of persons in that location, duration of training sessions (e.g. 09:00-12:00 and 13:00-15:00) as provided by the athletic staff, and sleep variables from the activity monitors as collected via the Readiband during this study (Balkin et al., 2000). These variables provide a measures of alertness derived from the SAFTE algorithm and have been correlated with and validated against the psychomotor-vigilance test (PVT) (R2 = 0.88,
Figure 1
Example of SAFTE graphical output of alertness

This study was based around an evening home game (19:00-21:00) in the Super Rugby competition during April 2015. This game was selected as no travel occurred for 13 days prior to, or 10 days after the game. Consequently, players had access to their usual home sleeping environment and performance was unaffected by travel across time zones. Continuous sleep measurements were obtained on each player over a 7-night period: pre-game (Wednesday, Thursday and Friday), the night of the game (Saturday) and post-game (Sunday, Monday, Tuesday). For data analysis, the 36 players were separated into those selected to play in the game (
Comparisons of demographics and sleep history measures were made between the two groups (
In the
Demographic, anthropometric measures and sleep history were similar between groups (Table 1). The number of days with disrupted sleep during the week was greater in the
Descriptive characteristics (mean ± SD or counts) of Game vs Non-Game Group
Game (n = 20) | Non‐ (Game n = 13) | |
---|---|---|
Age (years) | 26 ± 3 | 25 ± 3 |
Body Mass (kg) | 102 ± 12 | 102 ± 10 |
Height (cm) | 185 ± 7 | 185 ± 8 |
Body Mass Index (BMI) | 30 ± 3 | 30 ± 2 |
Neck size (cm) | 41 ± 6 | 45 ± 2 |
Sleep you feel you need each night (min) | 433 ± 52 | 431 ± 56 |
Sleep you get each night (min) | 426 ± 60 | 420 ± 58 |
Sleep you feel you need after training or competition (mins) | 465 ± 92 | 474 ± 72 |
348 ± | ||
Sleep you get after training or competition (mins) | 370 ± 122 | |
Number of days disrupted sleep per week (count) | 2 ± 2* | 4 ± 1 |
Number of times sleep is disrupted each night (count) | 2 ± 1 | 1 ± 1 |
Not important at all | - | - |
Somewhat important | 1 | 1 |
Important | 6 | 5 |
Extremely important | 9 | 7 |
Excellent | 4 | 3 |
Very Good | 10 | 5 |
Good | 2 | 2 |
Married | 2 | 2 |
Single | 9 | 3 |
Living with a partner | 5 | 3 |
Number of players with children under 18 (living at home) | 4 | 4 |
Considering the entire squad (n = 33) there was: a later time of sleep onset (~3 hrs) on the night of the game relative to all nights before and after the game; a tendency to progressively wake later leading up to a game (~2 hrs); and a progressive increase in sleep duration on the days leading up to a game (~1.5 hrs).
Figure 2

Figure 3

This study found that professional rugby players, those who were selected to play (compared to those not selected but who still attended the game), progressively increased sleep duration in the nights leading into an evening game. Those who played went to bed 3 hrs later on the night of the game (02:00 vs 23:00 hrs on other nights), with a resultant decrease in sleep duration of ~2.5 hrs on this night. These changes in sleep did not affect the modelled estimate of alertness, which remained above 93% of maximum during the game and all training sessions.
The progressive increase in sleep duration seen in the nights leading into an evening game has also been reported in two other rugby studies (Eagles et al., 2014; Shearer et al., 2015) and may represent an intentional pre-game strategy by athletes to optimise game performance. Our study shows that such sleep behaviours occur only in those players selected to play, and not in unselected players who attended the game as spectators. While the precise performance benefits of increases in sleep duration remain unclear, a previous study of prolonged periods of sleep extension in collegiate basketball players resulted in improved sprint times, shooting accuracy, reaction time and levels of daytime sleepiness (Mah et al., 2011). Similar sleep prolongation strategies are employed in military special operations by the intentional provision of extended sleep opportunities prior to deployment on missions (Yarnell and Deuster, 2016).
In contrast to the nights leading into a game, which were characterised by a progressive increase in sleep duration, on the game night, those who played the game had a 2.5-hr decrease in sleep duration. This was primarily due to a 3-hr delay in sleep onset time compared to all other nights. Such findings are consistent with results from other studies conducted in rugby union and the Australian Football League; after evening games, sleep onset time was delayed and sleep duration reduced compared to pre-game nights (Lastella et al., 2015; Sargent and Roach, 2016; Shearer et al., 2015). The delay in time to sleep following a game could be due to several factors, including media commitments, recovery sessions, socialising, persisting game-related increases in cortisol levels and mood disturbances, both of which can occur for 12-36 hrs after a rugby game (West et al., 2014) and up to 48 hrs following a rugby league game (McLellan et al., 2011). It was of interest that four players did not achieve any sleep after the game. The self-reported reasons for this included the effects of post-game arousal, the stress of having lost the game, and engagement in post-game socialising with other team members.
The mechanisms underlying the significant increase in time at sleep onset following an evening game still remain unclear but could include the potentially detrimental effects of pre-game ergogenic caffeine ingestion on sleep.
The nights following the game were characterised by a gradual return to pre-game sleep patterns. On the second and third nights, post-game wake times were constrained by scheduled early morning training sessions (commencing before 08:00). These early morning training sessions truncate the opportunity for sleep, thus reducing sleep duration (Dunican et al., 2017).
As previously noted, the measure of alertness used in this study has been validated against the PVT (R2 = 0.88, p ≤ 0.001) (Roma, 2012; Van Dongen, 2004), a test of sustained attention that is sensitive to sleep loss (Dawson and Reid, 1997) and circadian misalignment and which provides a surrogate measure of behavioural alertness (Van Dongen, 2004). The alertness algorithm and association with PVT has been validated in both the aviation and rail transportation industries (Hursh et al., 2006; Roma et al., 2012) such that values of alertness ≤90% are considered to reflect impaired reaction times. Indeed, alertness values between 80-90% have been associated with an 18% decrease in reaction time based upon PVT results and an alertness score of 77% has been shown to be accompanied by a 34% reduction in reaction time (Hursh et al., 2006; Roma et al., 2012), the latter decrease in reaction time being equivalent to that occurring with blood alcohol concentration of 0.05% or after 17 hrs of sustained wakefulness (Dawson and Reid, 1997). While there are no published reports of alertness being used in athletic populations, such information may be useful for identifying periods of cognitive impairment, which may result in suboptimal performance.
In the present study for both the Game and Non-Group players, alertness remained >90% during all training sessions and >95% during the game. The tendency for game alertness to increase might be attributable to increased sleep in the days before the game, particularly in those players who were selected to participate in the evening game. Potentially, substantial differences in alertness could occur with international travel, which occurs regularly with Super Rugby teams. By adjusting for changes in time zones, the SAFTE model used in this study (Hursh et al., 2004) and other bio-mathematical models could be used to predict optimal travel and training times for travelling teams.
The sample size in this study was limited by squad numbers (n = 36), as this was the only team based within the State. However, the data collected reflect the responses of the whole team, thus the findings are representative of sleep in elite rugby players within a Super Rugby team. Also, the results relate to a single Super Rugby home game with no interstate or international travel in the week before or after, as such it is not known how representative they are of other teams, especially following travel to away games.
Those professional rugby players who were selected during the week to play in an evening game on the weekend showed a progressive increase in sleep duration in the days leading up to the game. It is possible that such behaviour is an attempt by the player to maximise alertness for training and game time, which were high in the Game-Group. However, it was notable that high levels of alertness were also observed in those players who were not selected to play in the weekend game, suggesting that they might be prepared, at least in terms of alertness, for last-minute inclusion in the team on the game day if needed. The finding of a significant delay in time of sleep onset and reduction in sleep duration after a game in all players should be of interest to coaches, who could consider delaying post-game training sessions to allow for optimal recovery. Bio-mathematical modelling of alertness, as used in this study, may represent a useful tool for coaching and performance staff for the development of objective performance decisions related to training and recovery periods. Such modelling could, for example, be used to assist with scheduling of travel.
Figure 1

Figure 2

Figure 3

Descriptive characteristics (mean ± SD or counts) of Game vs Non-Game Group
Game (n = 20) | Non‐ (Game n = 13) | |
---|---|---|
Age (years) | 26 ± 3 | 25 ± 3 |
Body Mass (kg) | 102 ± 12 | 102 ± 10 |
Height (cm) | 185 ± 7 | 185 ± 8 |
Body Mass Index (BMI) | 30 ± 3 | 30 ± 2 |
Neck size (cm) | 41 ± 6 | 45 ± 2 |
Sleep you feel you need each night (min) | 433 ± 52 | 431 ± 56 |
Sleep you get each night (min) | 426 ± 60 | 420 ± 58 |
Sleep you feel you need after training or competition (mins) | 465 ± 92 | 474 ± 72 |
348 ± | ||
Sleep you get after training or competition (mins) | 370 ± 122 | |
Number of days disrupted sleep per week (count) | 2 ± 2* | 4 ± 1 |
Number of times sleep is disrupted each night (count) | 2 ± 1 | 1 ± 1 |
Not important at all | - | - |
Somewhat important | 1 | 1 |
Important | 6 | 5 |
Extremely important | 9 | 7 |
Excellent | 4 | 3 |
Very Good | 10 | 5 |
Good | 2 | 2 |
Married | 2 | 2 |
Single | 9 | 3 |
Living with a partner | 5 | 3 |
Number of players with children under 18 (living at home) | 4 | 4 |
Relationship among the Change of Direction Ability, Sprinting, Jumping Performance, Aerobic Power and Anaerobic Speed Reserve: A Cross-Sectional Study in Elite 3x3 Basketball Players Construct Validity and Applicability of a Team-Sport-Specific Change of Direction Test Change of Direction Deficit: A Promising Method to Measure a Change of Direction Ability in Adolescent Basketball Players Effects of Arm Dominance and Decision Demands on Change of Direction Performance in Handball Players Effectiveness and Kinematic Analysis of Initial Step Patterns for Multidirectional Acceleration in Team and Racquet Sports Change of Direction Ability as a Sensitive Marker of Adaptation to Different Training Configurations, and Different Populations: Results from Four Experiments Lower Limb Skeletal Robustness Determines the Change of Directional Speed Performance in Youth Ice Hockey Reactive Agility in Competitive Young Volleyball Players: A Gender Comparison of Perceptual-Cognitive and Motor Determinants The Relationship among Acceleration, Deceleration and Changes of Direction in Repeated Small Sided Games Change of Direction Performance and its Physical Determinants Among Young Basketball Male Players Training to Improve Pro-Agility Performance: A Systematic Review Relationships between Sprint, Acceleration, and Deceleration Metrics with Training Load in Division I Collegiate Women’s Soccer Players